Abstract
The distinction between basic sciences and clinical knowledge which has led to a theoretical debate on how medical expertise is developed has implications for medical school and lifelong medical education. This longitudinal, population based observational study was conducted to test the fit of three theories—knowledge encapsulation, independent influence, distinct domains—of the development of medical expertise employing structural equation modelling. Data were collected from 548 physicians (292 men—53.3%; 256 women—46.7%; mean age = 24.2 years on admission) who had graduated from medical school 2009–2014. They included (1) Admissions data of undergraduate grade point average and Medical College Admission Test sub-test scores, (2) Course performance data from years 1, 2, and 3 of medical school, and (3) Performance on the NBME exams (i.e., Step 1, Step 2 CK, and Step 3). Statistical fit indices (Goodness of Fit Index—GFI; standardized root mean squared residual—SRMR; root mean squared error of approximation—RSMEA) and comparative fit \((X_{D}^{2} ,X^{2} )\) of three theories of cognitive development of medical expertise were used to assess model fit. There is support for the knowledge encapsulation three factor model of clinical competency (GFI = 0.973, SRMR = 0.043, RSMEA = 0.063) which had superior fit indices to both the independent influence and distinct domains theories (\(X_{29}^{2} = 88.11\) vs \(X_{29}^{2} = 443.91\) [\(X_{D}^{2} = 355.80\)] vs \(X_{29}^{2} = 514.93\) [\(X_{D}^{2} = 426.82\)], respectively). The findings support a theory where basic sciences and medical aptitude are direct, correlated influences on clinical competency that encapsulates basic knowledge.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.



References
Baghdady, M. T., Carnahan, H., Lam, E. W., & Woods, N. N. (2013). Integration of basic sciences and clinical sciences in oral radiology education for dental students. Journal of Dental Education, 77(6), 757–763.
Baghdady, M. T., Carnahan, H., Lam, E., & Woods, N. N. (2014). Test-enhanced learning and its effect on comprehension and diagnostic accuracy. Medical Education, 48, 181–188.
Baghdady, M. T., Pharoah, M. J., Regher, G., Lam, E. W., & Woods, N. N. (2009). The role of basic sciences in diagnostic radiology. Journal of Dental Education, 73, 1187–1193.
Bollen, K. A. (1989). Structural equations with latent variables, Wiley series in probability and mathematical statistics. New York: Wiley.
Boshuizen, H. P., & Schmidt, H. G. (1992). On the role of biomedical knowledge in clinical reasoning by experts, intermediates and novices. Cognitive Science, 16, 153–184.
Collin, T., Violato, C., & Hecker, K. (2008). Aptitude, achievement and competence in medicine: A latent variable path model. Advances in Health Science Education, 14, 355–366.
De Bruin, A. B. H., Schmidt, H. G., & Rikers, R. M. J. P. (2005). The role of basic science knowledge and clinical knowledge in diagnostic reasoning: A structural equation modeling approach. Academic Medicine, 80, 765–773.
Dong, T., Swygert, K. A., Durning, S. J., Saguil, A., Gilliland, W. A., Cruess, D., et al. (2014). Validity evidence for medical school OSCEs: Associations with USMLE Step assessments. Teaching and Learning in Medicine, 26(4), 379–386.
Donnon, T., Oddone, E., & Violato, C. (2007). The predictive validity of the MCAT for medical school performance and medical board licensing examinations: A meta-analysis of the published research. Academic Medicine, 82, 100–106.
Donnon, T., & Violato, C. (2006). Medical students’ clinical reasoning skills as a function of basic science achievement and clinical competency measures: A structural equation model. Academic Medicine, 81(Suppl), S120–S123.
Hopkins, R., Pratt, D., Bowen, J., & Regehr, G. (2015). Integrating basic science without integrating basic scientists: Reconsidering the place of individual teachers in curriculum reform. Academic Medicine, 90, 149–153.
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indices in covariance structural analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1–55.
Kulasegaram, K. M., Manzone, J. C., Ku, C., Skye, A., Wadey, V., & Woods, N. N. (2015). Cause and effect: Testing a mechanism and method for the cognitive integration of basic science. Academic Medicine, 90(Nov suppl), S63–S69.
Kulasegaram, K. M., Martimianakis, M. A., Mylopoulos, M., Whitehead, C. R., & Woods, N. N. (2013). Cognition before curriculum: Rethinking the integration of basic science and clinical learning. Academic Medicine, 88, 1578–1585.
Lisk, K., Agur, A. M., & Woods, N. N. (2016a). Exploring cognitive integration of basic sciences and its effect on diagnostic reasoning. Perspectives in Medical Education, 5, 147–153.
Lisk, K., Agur, A. M., & Woods, N. N. (2016b). Examining the effect of self-explanation on cognitive integration of basic and clinical sciences in novices. In Advances in Health Sciences Education, 24 December.
Medical Council of Canada. http://mcc.ca/examinations. Accessed 29 June 2016.
National Board of Medical Examiners. http://www.usmle.org/. Accessed 25 June 2016.
Patel, V. L., Arocha, J. F., & Kaufman, D. R. (1994). Diagnostic reasoning and expertise. In D. L. Medin (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (pp. 187–252). San Diego: Academic Press.
Patel, V. L., Evans, D. A., & Groen, G. J. (1989). Reconciling basic science and clinical reasoning. Teaching and Learning in Medicine, 1, 116–121.
Patel, V. L., & Kaufman, D. R. (2000). Clinical reasoning and biomedical knowledge: Implications for teaching. In J. Higgs & M. Jones (Eds.), Clinical reasoning in the health professions (pp. 117–128). Oxford: Butterworth Heinemann.
Schmidt, H. G., & Boshuizen, H. P. (1993). On acquiring expertise in medicine. Educational and Psychological Review, 5, 205–221.
Schmidt, H. G., Norman, G. R., & Boshuizen, H. P. (1990). A cognitive perspective on medical expertise: Theory and implications. Academic Medicine, 65, 611–621.
Schmidt, H. G., & Rikers, R. M. (2007). How expertise develops in medicine: Knowledge encapsulation and illness script formation. Medical Education, 41, 1133–1139.
Violato, C., & Donnon, T. (2005). Does the Medical College Admission Test predict clinical reasoning skills? A longitudinal study employing the Medical Council of Canada Clinical Reasoning Examination. Academic Medicine, 80(10 suppl), S14–S16.
Violato, C., & Hecker, K. G. (2007). How to use structural equation modeling in medical education research: A brief guide. Teaching and Learning in Medicine, 19, 362–371.
Violato, C., Shen, E., & Gao, H. (2016). Does Step 3 of the United States Medical Licensing Exam Measure Clinical Competence? A Predictive Validity Study. Medical Science Educator. doi:10.1007/s40670-016-0263-6->.
Woods, N. N. (2007). Science is fundamental: The role of biomedical knowledge in clinical reasoning. Medical Education, 41, 1173–1177.
Woods, N. N., Brooks, L. R., & Norman, G. R. (2005). The value of basic science in clinical diagnosis: Creating coherence among signs and symptoms. Medical Education, 39, 107–112.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Violato, C., Gao, H., O’Brien, M.C. et al. How do physicians become medical experts? A test of three competing theories: distinct domains, independent influence and encapsulation models. Adv in Health Sci Educ 23, 249–263 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10459-017-9784-z
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10459-017-9784-z
Keywords
- Clinical reasoning
- Assessment
- Encapsulation theory
- Medical expertise