Skip to main content
Log in

“This is a Titanic song”: the effect of familiarity on children’s learning affective meaning in music

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Cognitive Processing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Usage-based theories of language acquisition are thought to rely on domain-general learning mechanisms, such as mastering familiar routines by rote before generalizing to novel unfamiliar instances. If so, then the role of familiarity should extend to non-linguistic domains, like music. The purpose of the present study was to test the role of familiarity in children’s learning of affective meaning of music. Music carries an affective meaning that is relayed through its elements, such as mode, rhythm, and tempo. The previous research has found differences between children and adults in their understanding of music’s affective meaning, suggesting that this meaning is learned. We predicted that children would initially learn the affective meaning of familiar musical pieces before generalizing to unfamiliar pieces. Children between 3 and 5 years of age heard 16 musical segments, one for each emotion (i.e., anger, sadness, and happiness), and their accuracy in pairing music–emotion was measured. For younger children, their familiarity with the piece was positively associated with their accuracy. These results suggest that familiarity plays a role in learning affective meaning in music, providing support for the claim that this learning mechanism is domain-general.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abbot-Smith K, Tomasello M (2006) Exemplar-learning and schematization in a usage-based account of syntactic acquisition. Linguist Rev 23:273–288

    Google Scholar 

  • Adachi M, Trehub SE (1998) Children’s expression of emotion in song. Psychol Music 26:133–153

    Google Scholar 

  • Adachi M, Trehub SE (2000) Decoding the expressive intentions in children’s songs. Music Percept Interdiscip J 18:213–224

    Google Scholar 

  • Adachi M, Trehub SE, Abe JI (2004) Perceiving emotion in children’s songs across age and culture. Jpn Psychol Res 46:322–336

    Google Scholar 

  • Adachi M, Trehub SE (2011) Canadian and Japanese preschoolers’ creation of happy and sad songs. Psychomusicol Music Mind Brain 21(1–2):130–143

    Google Scholar 

  • Ambridge B, Rowland CF, Pine JM, Young CR (2008) The effect of verb semantic class and verb frequency (entrenchment) on children’s and adults’ graded judgements of argument-structure overgeneralization errors. Cognition 106:87–129

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ambridge B, Kidd E, Rowland CF, Theakston AL (2015) The ubiquity of frequency effects in first language acquisition. J Child Lang 42:239–273

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Balkwill LL, Thompson WF (1999) A cross-cultural investigation of the perception of emotion in music: psychophysical and cultural cues. Music Percept 17:43–64

    Google Scholar 

  • Balkwill LL, Thompson WF, Matsunaga RIE (2004) Recognition of emotion in Japanese, Western, and Hindustani music by Japanese listeners. Jpn Psychol Res 46:337–349

    Google Scholar 

  • Bannard C, Matthews D (2008) Stored word sequences in language learning: the effect of familiarity on children’s repetition of four-word combinations. Psychol Sci 19:241–248

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baumgartner T, Lutz K, Schmidt CF, Jäncke L (2005) The emotional power of music: how music enhances the feeling of affective pictures. Brain Res 1075:151–164

    Google Scholar 

  • Blood AJ, Zatorre RJ, Bermudez P, Evans AC (1999) Emotional responses to pleasant and unpleasant music correlate with activity in paralimbic brain regions. Nat Neurosci 2:382–387

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brechet C, Baldy R, Picard D (2009) How does Sam feel?: Children’s labelling and drawing of basic emotions. Br J Dev Psychol 27:587–606

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bybee JL, Beckner C (2010) Usage-based theory. In: Heine B, Narrog H (eds) The Oxford handbook of linguistic analysis. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 827–855

    Google Scholar 

  • Camras LA, Allison K (1985) Children’s understanding of emotional facial expressions and verbal labels. J Nonverbal Behav 9:84–94

    Google Scholar 

  • Christiansen MH, Chater N (2015) The language faculty that wasn’t: a usage-based account of natural language recursion. Front Psychol 6:1182

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Costa-Giomi E (1996) Mode discrimination abilities of pre-school children. Psychol Music 24:184–198

    Google Scholar 

  • Demany L, McKenzie B, Vurpillot E (1997) Rhythm perception in early infancy. Nature 266:718–719

    Google Scholar 

  • Eerola T, Friberg A, Bresin R (2013) Emotional expression in music: contribution, linearity, and additivity of primary musical cues. Front Psychol 4:487

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gao X, Maurer D (2009) Influence of intensity on children’s sensitivity to happy sad and fearful facial expressions. J Exp Child Psychol 102:503–521

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg AE (2006) Constructions at work: the nature of generalization in language. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Gregory AH, Worrall L, Sarge A (1996) The development of emotional responses to music in young children. Motiv Emot 20:341–348

    Google Scholar 

  • Hannon EE, Johnson SP (2005) Infants use meter to categorise rhythms and melodies: implications for musical structure learning. Cogn Psychol 50:354–377

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hunter PG, Schellenberg EG, Stalinski SM (2011) Liking and identifying emotionally expressive music: age and gender differences. J Exp Child Psychol 110:80–93

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ibbotson P (2013) The scope of usage-based theory. Front Psychol 4:255

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Juslin PN, Laukka P (2004) Expression, perception, and induction of musical emotions: a review and a questionnaire study of everyday listening. J New Music Res 33:217–238

    Google Scholar 

  • Karmiloff-Smith A (2010) Multiple trajectories to human language acquisition: domain-specific or domain-general? Hum Dev 53:239–249

    Google Scholar 

  • Kastner MP, Crowder RG (1990) Perception of the major/minor distinction: IV. Emotional connotations in young children. Music Percept 8:189–201

    Google Scholar 

  • Koelsch S, Kasper E, Sammler D, Schulze K, Gunter T, Friederici AD (2004) Music language and meaning: brain signatures of semantic processing. Nat Neurosci 7:302–307

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl PK (2004) Early language acquisition: cracking the speech code. Nat Rev Neurosci 5:831–843

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lieven E (2014) First language development: a usage-based perspective on past and current research. J Child Lang 41:48–63

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lieven EVM, Behrens H, Speares J, Tomasello M (2003) Early syntactic creativity: a usage-based approach. J Child Lang 30:333–370

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Matthews D, Lieven E, Theakston A, Tomasello M (2005) The role of frequency in the acquisition of English word order. Cogn Dev 20:121–136

    Google Scholar 

  • Nawrot ES (2003) The perception of emotional expression in music: evidence from infants children and adults. Psychol Music 31:75–92

    Google Scholar 

  • Paradis J, Crago M, Genesee F (2006) Domain-general versus domain-specific accounts of specific language impairment: evidence from bilingual children’s acquisition of object pronouns. Lang Acquis 13:33–62

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner J, Russell JA, Peterson BS (2005) The circumplex model of affect: an integrative approach to affective neuroscience, cognitive development, and psychopathology. Dev Psychopathol 17:715–734

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Saarikallio S, Tervaniemi M, Yrtti A, Huotilainen M (2019) Expression of emotion through musical parameters in 3-and 5-year-olds. Music Educ Res 21:596–605

    Google Scholar 

  • Schubert E, McPherson GE (2016) Underlying mechanisms and processes in the development of emotion perception in music. In: McPherson GE (ed) The child as musician: a handbook of musical development. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 221–243

    Google Scholar 

  • Stachó L, Saarikallio S, Van Zijl A, Huotilainen M, Toiviainen P (2013) Perception of emotional content in musical performances by 3–7-year-old children. Musicae Scientia 17:495–512

    Google Scholar 

  • Stefanics G, Háden GP, Sziller I, Balázs L, Beke A, Winkler I (2009) Newborn infants process pitch intervals. Clin Neurophysiol 120:304–308

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tan L, Smith CL (2018) Function of child anger and sadness in response to a blocked goal. J Exp Child Psychol 170:190–196

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor C (2016) The language animal: the full shape of the human linguistic capacity. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Terwogt MM, Van Grinsven F (1991) Musical expression of moodstates. Psychol Music 19:99–109

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello M (2000) Do young children have adult syntactic competence? Cognition 74:209–253

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello M (2003) Constructing a language: a usage-based theory of language acquisition. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Trehub SE, Hannon EE, Schachner A (2010) Perspectives on music and affect in the early years. In: Juslin PN, Sloboda J (eds) Handbook of music and emotion: theory, research, applications. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 645–668

    Google Scholar 

  • Widen SC, Russell JA (2004) The relative power of an emotion’s facial expression, label, and behavioral consequence to evoke preschoolers’knowledge of its cause. Cogn Dev 19(1):111–125

    Google Scholar 

  • Widen SC, Russell JA (2008) Children acquire emotion categories gradually. Cogn Dev 23:291–312

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

This study was funded by a Discovery Grant from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada to the second author (Grant Number 2018-04978).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Elena Nicoladis.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the TriCouncil policy of Canada and the University of Alberta and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Additional information

Handling Editor: Mari Tervaniemi (University of Helsinki).

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Reviewers: Swathi Swaminathan (University of Toronto) and a second researcher who prefers to remain unanonymous.

Appendices

Appendix A

Musical elements of segments

 

Anger

Happiness

Sadness

Maleficent’s Frustration

(0:10–0:27)

Sleeping Beauty (1959)

Intense articulations, repetitive dramatic rhythms, quick dynamic changes, low pitches

  

Have a Bite

(0:39–1:07)

Snow White and the Seven Dwarfs (1937)

Minor mode, fast paced rhythms, slow crescendo in percussion to the climax, hard accents

  

Sinister Stromboli

(1:05–1:21)

Pinocchio (1940)

No clear melody, fast rhythms, changing tempo, sharp crescendos, low brass

  

McQueen’s Lost

(1:23–1:38)

Cars (2006)

Minor mode, low brass instruments, harsh attacks in accents, intense dynamic changes, changes in pitch

  

Ewe Fell For It

(0:50–1:06)

Zootopia (2016)

Atonality, sharp crescendos, dramatic dynamics, harsh accents, low pitches, low brass instruments primarily

  

Whiteout

(0:52–1:09)

Frozen (2013)

Heavy accents, immediate crescendos, fast tempo, sudden pitch changes

  

Little Wooden Head

(1:03–1:20)

Pinocchio (1940)

 

Major mode, fast tempo and rhythm, clear melody, high pitches, constant dynamic, high woodwinds

 

Love’s First Kiss/Finale

(2:33–2:50)

Snow White and the Seven Dwarfs (1937)

 

Major modality, quick tempo, moving rhythms, higher pitches, constant dynamics

 

The Three Good Fairies

(0:08–0:23)

Sleeping Beauty (1959)

 

Major mode, high woodwinds, clear melody, fast tempo, high pitches

 

Suite from Zootopia

(3:22–3:40)

Zootopia (2016)

 

Major mode, quick rhythmic pattern, higher pitches, dynamic consistency

 

New Road

(0:01–0:20)

Cars (2006)

 

Clear melody, major mode, high pitches, consistent dynamics, high woodwinds primarily

 

Elsa and Anna

(0:29–0:40)

Frozen (2013)

 

Major mode, smooth rhythms, constant upbeat tempo, simple bass line, higher woodwinds, high pitches

 

Poor Aurora/Sleeping Beauty

(0:27–0:45)

Sleeping Beauty (1959)

  

Minor mode, strings and solo flute, high and low pitches, soft dynamics, slow moving rhythm

Sad Reunion

(1:40–2:00)

Pinocchio (1940)

  

Minor, tempo rises and falls, stringed instruments, high pitches, slow moving rhythms

Animal Friends

(0:18–0:38)

Snow White and the Seven Dwarfs (1937)

  

Minor mode string instruments primarily, softer dynamics, slow rhythmic patterns, low pitches

Goodbye

(0:15–0:35)

Cars (2006)

  

Minor mode, slow tempo, solo instruments, thin texture, legato rhythms

Not a Real Cop

(0:29–0:52)

Zootopia (2016)

  

Minor mode, slow moving rhythm, few instruments, piano, quiet dynamics, legato articulations

Only an Act of True Love

(0:21–0:38)

Frozen (2013)

  

Solo oboe line, minor mode, quiet dynamics, thin texture, slow tempo and rhythms

Appendix B

Emojis used for pairings (in order from top left to bottom right: angry, sad, happy, surprised).

figure a

Appendix C

Confusability matrices for younger and older children

 

Familiar

 

Unfamiliar

Selected emotion

Selected emotion

Angry

Happy

Sad

Surprised

na

Total

Angry

Happy

Sad

Surprised

na

Total

Angry

22

2

6

3

12

45

Angry

8

5

2

2

13

30

Happy

5

28

1

5

12

51

Happy

9

16

6

12

19

62

Sad

5

5

22

4

15

51

Sad

7

11

10

9

25

62

Total

32

35

29

12

39

147

Total

24

32

18

23

57

154

  1. Younger children’s selected emotions by target emotions. Bold indicates accurate choices
 

Familiar

 

Unfamiliar

Selected emotion

Selected emotion

Angry

Happy

Sad

Surprised

na

Total

Angry

Happy

Sad

Surprised

na

Total

Angry

42

5

2

3

1

53

Angry

14

2

0

0

2

18

Happy

5

47

6

6

1

65

Happy

5

21

4

8

4

42

Sad

3

13

44

5

1

66

Sad

2

3

26

7

4

42

Total

50

65

52

14

3

184

Total

21

26

30

15

10

102

  1. Older children’s selected emotions by target emotions. Bold indicates accurate choices

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Stuber, R., Nicoladis, E. “This is a Titanic song”: the effect of familiarity on children’s learning affective meaning in music. Cogn Process 22, 105–116 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-020-00988-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-020-00988-2

Navigation