Skip to main content
Log in

Familiarity and preference for pitch probability profiles

  • Short Report
  • Published:
Cognitive Processing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We investigated familiarity and preference judgments of participants toward a novel musical system. We exposed participants to tone sequences generated from a novel pitch probability profile. Afterward, we either asked participants to identify more familiar or we asked participants to identify preferred tone sequences in a two-alternative forced-choice task. The task paired a tone sequence generated from the pitch probability profile they had been exposed to and a tone sequence generated from another pitch probability profile at three levels of distinctiveness. We found that participants identified tone sequences as more familiar if they were generated from the same pitch probability profile which they had been exposed to. However, participants did not prefer these tone sequences. We interpret this relationship between familiarity and preference to be consistent with an inverted U-shaped relationship between knowledge and affect. The fact that participants identified tone sequences as even more familiar if they were generated from the more distinctive (caricatured) version of the pitch probability profile which they had been exposed to suggests that the statistical learning of the pitch probability profile is involved in gaining of musical knowledge.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

References

  • Berlyne DE (1974) The new experimental aesthetics. In: Berlyne DE (ed) Studies in the new experimental aesthetics: steps towards an objective psychology of aesthetic appreciation. Hemisphere Publishing Co., Washington, pp 1–25

    Google Scholar 

  • Cantor GN (1968) Children’s ‘‘like–dislike’’ ratings of familiarized and unfamiliarized visual stimuli. J Exp Child Psychol 6:651–657

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huron (2006) Sweet anticipation. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Huron D, Veltman J (2006) A cognitive approach to medieval mode: evidence for an historical antecedent to the major/minor system. Empir Musicol Rev 1:33–55

    Google Scholar 

  • Krumhansl CL (1985) Perceiving tonal structure in music. Am Sci 73:371–378

    Google Scholar 

  • Krumhansl CL (1990) Cognitive foundations of musical pitch. Oxford University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Krumhansl CL, Cuddy LL (2010) A theory of tonal hierarchies in music. In: Jones MR, Fay RR, Popper AN (eds) Music perception. Springer, New York, pp 51–87

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn G, Dienes Z (2005) Implicit learning of nonlocal musical rules: implicitly learning more than chunks. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 31:1417–1432

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loui P, Wessel DL, Hudson Kam CL (2010) Humans rapidly learn grammatical structure in a new musical scale. Music Percept 27:327–377

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martindale C, Moore K (1989) Relationship of musical preference to collative, ecological, and psychophysical variables. Music Percept 6:431–445

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martindale C, Moore K, Borkum J (1990) Aesthetic preference: Anomalous findings for Berlyne’s psychobiological theory. Am J Psychol 105:53–80

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer LB (1956) Emotion and meaning in music. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Peretz I, Gaudreau D, Bonnel AM (1998) Exposure effects on music preference and recognition. Mem Cognit 26:884–902

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schellenberg EG, Peretz I, Vieillard S (2008) Liking for happy-and sad-sounding music: effects of exposure. Cogn Emot 22:218–237

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seamon JG, Williams PC, Crowley MJ, Kim IJ, Langer SA, Orne PJ, Wishengrad DL (1995) The mere exposure effect is based on implicit memory: effects of stimulus type, encoding conditions, and number of exposures on recognition and affect judgments. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 21:711–721

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith JD, Melara RJ (1990) Aesthetic preference and syntactic prototypicality in music: T is the gift to be simple. Cognition 34:279–298

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith NA, Schmuckler MA (2004) The perception of tonal structure through the differentiation and organization of pitches. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 30:268–286

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Szpunar KK, Schellenberg EG, Pliner P (2004) Liking and memory for musical stimuli as a function of exposure. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 30:370–381

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Temperley D (2007) Music and probability. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Tillmann B, Bharucha JJ, Bigand E (2000) Implicit learning of tonality: a self-organizing approach. Psychol Rev 107:885–913

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zajonc RB (1968) Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. J Pers Soc Psychol 9:1–27

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by a Discovery Grant from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Council of Canada to LLC. We would like to thank four anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Anja-Xiaoxing Cui.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Cui, AX., Collett, M.J., Troje, N.F. et al. Familiarity and preference for pitch probability profiles. Cogn Process 16, 211–218 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-015-0651-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-015-0651-7

Keywords

Navigation