Skip to main content
Log in

Dynamic sensory–motor oscillation and cerebral development

  • Review
  • Published:
Cognitive Processing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Drawing from Freud’s Project, the author proposes a model of cerebral development whose sensory–motor structure is defined by a frontal–occipital oscillatory dynamic with a twofold function: the oscillation explains the formation and maintenance of mother–infant attunement in cerebral growth, while, at the same time, also explaining the functioning of the projective–introjective dynamic at the basis of psychoanalytic theory. The oscillatory dynamic, according to this perspective, operates as a “bridge” between two seminal theoretical models of developments—the psychoanalytic and the infant research model—which, in turn, leads to the formulation of some neurological hypotheses on how oscillation regulates the elaboration of maternal interaction in the infant’s brain, and how the mother may act to modify it. The paper discusses how the oscillatory dynamic offers an innovative framework for the reconceptualization of the development of mentalization, the function of mirror neurons, and, most interestingly, of the development of language, explaining the non-verbal properties of ordinary linguistic communication and the function of oscillation in the regulation of information exchange processing.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8
Fig. 9
Fig. 10
Fig. 11
Fig. 12
Fig. 13
Fig. 14
Fig. 15
Fig. 16
Fig. 17

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adolphs R (2003) Cognitive neuroscience of human social behavior. Nat Rev 4:165–178

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ainsworth MDS, Blehar M, Waters E, Wall S (1978) Patterns of attachment: a psychological study of the strange situation. Erlbaum, Hillsdale

    Google Scholar 

  • Anzieu D (1985) The skin ego. Yale University Press, New Haven

    Google Scholar 

  • Armstrong D (1968) A materialist theory of the mind. Routledge and Kegan Paul, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Astafiev SV, Shulman GL, Stanley CM, Snyder AZ, Van Essen DC, Corbetta M (2003) Functional organization of human intraparietal and frontal cortex for attending, looking, and pointing. J Neurosci 23:4689–4699

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ballard D (1991) Animate vision. Artif Intell 48:57–86

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen S (1995) Mindblindness: an essay on autism and theory of mind. MIT Press/Bradford Books, Cambridge, MA

  • Bartsch K, Wellman H (1995) Children talk about the mind. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Bateman A, Fonagy P (2004) Psychotherapy for borderline personality disorder mentalization-based treatment. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Battaglia-Mayer A, Caminiti R, Lacquaniti F, Zago M (2003) Multiple levels of representation of reaching in the parieto-frontal network. Cereb Cortex 13:1009–1022

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beebe B, Lachmann F (2002) Infant research and adult treatment: co-constructing interactions. The Analytic Press, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Belsky J, Spritz B, Crnic K (1996) Infant attachment security and affective-cognitive information processing at age 3. Psychol Sci 7:111–114

    Google Scholar 

  • Bick E (1968) The experience of the skin in early object relations. Int J Psychol Anal 49:484–486

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bion W (1955) The development of schizophrenic thought. Int J Psychol Anal 37:344–346

    Google Scholar 

  • Bion W (1963) Elements of psychoanalysis. William Heinemann, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Blair RJ, Cipollotti L (2000) Impaired social response reversal. A case of ‘acquired sociopathy’. Brain 123:1122–1141

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boris NW, Zeanah CH (1999) Reactive attachment disorder. In: Kaplan HI, Sadock BJ (eds) Comprehensive textbook of psychiatry. Williams & Wilkins, Philadelphia

    Google Scholar 

  • Bressler SL, Kelso JA (2001) Cortical coordination dynamics and cognition. Trend Cogn Sci 5:26–36

    Google Scholar 

  • Bucci W (1985) Dual coding: a cognitive model for psychoanalytic research. J Am Psychoanal Assoc 33:571–607

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Butler AB, Hodos W (2005) Comparative vertebrate neuroanatomy: evolution and adaptation, 2nd edn. John Wiley & Sons, Hoboken, NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • Chiron C, Jambaque I, Nabbot R, Lounes R, Syrota A, Dulac O (1997) The right brain is dominant in human infants. Brain 120:1057–1065

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Churchland PS, Ramachandran VS, Sejnowski TJ (1994) A critique of pure vision. In: Koch C, Davis J (eds) Large-scale neuronal theories of the brain. MIT Press, Boston, pp 23–60

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper RP, Shallice T (2000) Contention scheduling of the control of routine actions. Cogn Neuropsychol 17:293–338

    Google Scholar 

  • Couso JP (2009) Segmentation, metamerism and the Cambrian explosion. Int J Dev Biol 53(8), in press

  • Crittenden PM (1985) Maltreated infants: vulnerability and resilience. J Child Psychol Psychiatr 26(1):85–96

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Crittenden PM (1988) Relationships at risk. In: Belsky J, Nezworski T (eds) Clinical implication of attachment. Hillsdale, Erlbaum, pp 136–174

    Google Scholar 

  • Crittenden PM (1992) Quality of attachment in preschool years. Dev Psycopathol 4:209–241

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio AR (1999) The feeling of what happens: body and emotion in the making of consciousness. Harcourt Brace, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Denny-Brown D, Chambers RA (1958) The parietal lobe and behavior. Res Publ Assoc Nerv Ment Dis 36:35–117

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Deutsch H (1942) Some forms of emotional disturbance and their relationship to schizophrenia. Psychoanal Q 11:301–321

    Google Scholar 

  • Edelman GM, Tononi G (2000) A universe of consciousness: how matter becomes imagination. Basic Books, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Farah M (1989) The neural basis of mental imagery. TINS 12:395–399

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Field T (1985) Attachment as psychobiological attunement: being on the same wavelength. In: Reite LM, Field T (eds) The psychobiology of attachment and separation. Academic Press, Orlando, pp 415–454

    Google Scholar 

  • Fingelkurts ANA, Fingelkurts AlA, Neves CFH (2009) Phenomenological architecture of a mind and operational architectonics of the brain: the unified metastable continuum. J New Math Nat Comput 5(1):221–244

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischer KW (1980) A theory of cognitive development: the control and construction of hierarchies of skills. Psychol Rev 87:477–531

    Google Scholar 

  • Fonagy P (1999) Memory and therapeutic action. Int J Psychoanal 80:215–223

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Foss JE (1992) Introduction to the epistemology of the brain: indeterminacy, micro-specificity, chaos, and openness. Topoi 11:45–57

    Google Scholar 

  • Fosshage J (2005) The explicit and implicit domains in psychoanalytic change. Psychoanal Inq 25(4):516–539

    Google Scholar 

  • Fraiberg S (1980) Clinical studies in infant mental health. Tavistock, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Freeman WJ (1991) The physiology of perception. Sci Am 264:78–85

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Freeman WJ (1997) Non linear neurodynamics of intentionality. J Mind Behav 18(2–3):291–304

    Google Scholar 

  • Freeman WJ (1999) How brains make up their minds. Columbia University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Freeman WJ (2000) Neurodynamics. Springer, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Freud S (1895) Project for a scientific psychology. SE 1:295–391

  • Freud S (1900) The interpretation of dreams. Part I SE 4:1–338; Part II SE 5:339–625

  • Freud S (1905) Three essays on the theory of sexuality. SE 7:130–243

    Google Scholar 

  • Freud S (1914) On narcissism: an introduction. SE 14:73–102

    Google Scholar 

  • Freud S (1915) Instincts and their vicissitudes. SE 14:117–140

    Google Scholar 

  • Freud S (1920) Beyond the pleasure principle. SE 18:7–64

    Google Scholar 

  • Freud S (1922) The ego and the id. SE 19:12–66

    Google Scholar 

  • Gaddini E (1969) On imitation. Int J Psychol Anal 50:473–484

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallese V (1999) From grasping to language: mirror neurons and the origin of social communication. In: Hameroff SR, Kaszniak AW, Chalmers DJ (eds) Toward a science of consciousness III: the third Tucson discussion and debates. MIT Press, Boston, pp 165–178

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallese V (2003) The manifold nature of interpersonal relations: the quest for a common mechanism. Phil Trans Roy Soc Lond 358:517–528

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallese V, Keysers C, Rizzolatti G (2004) A unifying view of the basis of social cognition. Trends Cogn Sci 8:396–403

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Galletti C, Kutz DF, Gamberini M, Breveglieri R, Fattori P (2003) Role of the medial parieto-occipital cortex in the control of reaching and grasping movements. Exp Brain Res 153:158–170

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson JJ (1950) The perception of the visual world. Riverside press, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson JJ (1966) The senses considered as perceptual systems. Houghton Mifflin, Boston

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson JJ (1979) The ecological approach to visual perception. Houghton Mifflin, Boston

    Google Scholar 

  • Gopnik A (1993) How we know our minds: the illusion of first-person knowledge about intentionality. Behav Brain Sci 16:1–14

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray J (1995) The contents of consciousness: a neuropsychological conjecture. Behav Brain Sci 18:659–722

    Google Scholar 

  • Grossberg S (1988) Neural networks and natural intelligence. MIT Press, Boston

    Google Scholar 

  • Hilditch DJ (1995) At the heart of the world: Merleau-Ponty and the existential phenomenology of embodied and embedded intelligence in everyday coping. Unpublished Ph.D. Thesis. Washington University, St Louis, MO

  • Ishai A, Ungerleider LG, Haxby JV (2000) Distributed neural systems for the generation of visual images. Neuron 28:979–990

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jackson JH (1931) Evolution and dissolution of the nervous system. In: Taylor J (ed) Selected writings of John Hughlings Jackson, vol 2. Hodder and Stoughton, London, pp 92–118

  • Kandel ER, Schwartz JH, Jessell TM (1991) Principles of neural science, 3rd edn. Elsevier, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan-Solms K, Solms M (2000) Clinical studies in neuro-psychoanalysis: an introduction to depth neuropsychology. Other Press/Karnac Books, New York and London

    Google Scholar 

  • Kinsbourne M (1998) Unity and diversity in the human brain. Daedalus 127:233–256

    Google Scholar 

  • Klein M (1946) Notes on some schizoid mechanisms. Int J Psychoanal 27:99–110

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klein M (1957) Envy and gratitude: a study of unconscious forces. Basic Books, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn SM, Thompson WL (2003) When is early visual cortex activated during visual mental imagery? Psychol Bull Sep 129(5):723–746

    Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn SM (1994) Image and brain: the resolution of the imagery debate. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Leslie AM (1987) Pretense and representation: the origins of ‘theory of mind’. Psychol Rev 94:412–426

    Google Scholar 

  • Leslie AM (1991) The theory of mind impairment in autism: evidence for a modular mechanism of development? In: Whiten A (ed) Natural theories of mind: evolution, development and simulation of everyday mindreading. Basil Blackwell, Oxford, pp 65–78

    Google Scholar 

  • Livingston RB (1967) Brain circuity relating to complex behaviour. In: Schmitt FO, Quarton GC, Melnechuk T (eds) The neurosciences. Rockfeller University Press, New York, pp 568–576

    Google Scholar 

  • Llinas RR, Pare D (1996) The brain as a closed system modulated by the senses. In: Llinas R, Churchland P (eds) The mind-brain continuum. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 1–18

    Google Scholar 

  • Lumsden A, Krumlauf R (1996) Patterning the vertebrate neuraxis. Science 274:1109–1115

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lumsden A (2004) Segmentation and compartition in the early avian hindbrain. Mech Dev 121(9):1081–1088

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lyons-Ruth K (1998) Implicit relational knowing: its role in development and psychoanalytic treatment. Infant Ment Health J 19:282–289

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyons-Ruth K, Connell DB, Zoll D, Stahl J (1987) Infants at social risk: relationships among infant maltreatment maternal behavior and infant attachment behavior. Dev Psychol 23:223–232

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyons-Ruth K, Connell DB, Zoll D (1989) Patterns of maternal behavior among infants at risk for abuse: relations with Infant attachment behavior and Infant development at 12 months of age. In: Cicchetti D, Carlson V (eds) Child maltreatment, theory and research on the causes and consequences of child abuse and neglect. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 464–493

    Google Scholar 

  • Mahler MS (1952) On childhood psychosis and schizophrenia: autistic and symbiotic infantile psychoses. Psychoanal Study Child 7:268–305

    Google Scholar 

  • Mahler MS (1968) On human symbiosis and the vicissitudes of individuation. Vol. 1: infantile psychosis. International University Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Main M (1991) Metacognitive knowledge, metacognitive monitoring, and singular (coherent) versus multiple (incoherent) models of attachment. In: Parkes CM, Stevenson-Hinde J, Marris P (eds) Attachment across the life cycle. Routledge, London, pp 127–159

    Google Scholar 

  • Main M, Goldwyn R (l985–1996) Adult attachment scoring and classification system. Unpublished manuscript Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley

  • Main M, Solomon J (1986) Discovery of a new, insecure-disorganized/disoriented attachment pattern. In: Brazelton TB, Yogman M (eds) Affective development in infancy. Ablex, Norwood, pp 95–124

    Google Scholar 

  • Main M, Solomon J (1990) Procedure for identifying infants as disorganized/disoriented during the Ainsworth Strange Situation. In: Greenberg MT, Cicchetti D, Cummings EM (eds) Attachment in preschool years. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Markowitsch HJ (2000) The anatomical bases of memory. In: Gazzaniga MS (ed) The new cognitive neurosciences. The Mit Press, Cambridge, pp 781–795

    Google Scholar 

  • Meltzer D (1967) The psycho-analytical process. Heinemann, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Meltzer D (1973) Sexual states of mind. Clunie Press, Perthshire

    Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff A (2005) Imitation and other minds: the like me hypothesis. In: Hurley S, Chater N (eds) Perspectives on imitation: from neuroscience to social science, vol 2. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 55–57

    Google Scholar 

  • Misés R, Fortineau J, Jeammet P, Lang JL, Mazet PH, Plantade A, Quémada N (1988) Classification française des troubles mentaux de l’enfant et de l’adolescent. Psichiatrie de l’enfant 31:67–134

    Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell SA (2000) Relationality: from attachment to intersubjectivity. The Analytic Press, Hillsdale

    Google Scholar 

  • Modell A (1996) The interface of psychoanalysis and neurobiology. 12 Boston Colloquium for Philosophy of Science, 18 December

  • Mundy P, Sigman M, Kasari C (1995) La teoria della mente e i deficit dell’attenzione condivisa nell’autismo. In: Camaioni L (ed) La teoria della mente. Laterza, Bari

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicholls G, Van Essen D (1974) The nervous system of the leech. Sci Am 230:38–48

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Craven KM, Kanwisher N (2000) Mental imagery of faces and places activates corresponding stimulus-specific brain regions. J Cogn Neurosci 12:1013–1023

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ogden T (1989) The primitive edge of experience. Jason Aronson, Northvale

    Google Scholar 

  • Pally R (1998) Bilaterality: hemispheric specialisation and integration. Int J Psychol Anal 79:565–578

    Google Scholar 

  • Pally R (2000) The mind-brain relationship. Other Press/Karnac Books, New York and London

    Google Scholar 

  • Polan HJ, Hofer M (1999) Psycobiological origins of infant attachment and separation responses. In: Cassidy J, Shaver PR (eds) Handbook of attachment: theory, research and clinical applications. The Guilford press, New York, pp 162–180

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti G, Fogassi L, Gallese G (2001) Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nat Rev Neurosci 2:661–670

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti G, Matelli M (2003) Two different streams form the dorsal visual system: anatomy and functions. Exp Brain Res 153:146–157

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti G, Fadiga L, Fogassi L, Gallese V (1999) Resonance behaviors and mirror neurons. Arch Ital Biol 137:83–89

    Google Scholar 

  • Robertson SS (1987) Human cyclic motility: fetal-newborn continuities and newborn state differences. Dev Psychobiol 20:425–442

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robertson SS, Cohen AH, Mayer-Kress G (1993) Behavioral chaos: beyond the metaphor. In: Thelen E, Smith LB (eds) A dynamic systems approach to development: applications. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 119–150

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenfeld HA (1983) Primitive object relations and mechanisms. Int J Psychol Anal 64:261–267

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenfeld HA (1987) Impasse and interpretation: therapeutic and anti-therapeutic factors in the psychoanalytic treatment of psychotic, borderline, and neurotic patients. Routledge, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Sasso G (2005) Psicoanalisi e Neuroscienze. Astrolabio, Roma

    Google Scholar 

  • Sasso G (2007) The development of consciousness: an integrative model of child development. Neuroscience and Psychoanalysis, Karnac, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Schacter DL (1996) Searching for memory: the brain, the mind and the past. Basic Books, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Schore AN (1994) Affect regulation and the origin of the self. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale

    Google Scholar 

  • Schore AN (1999) The right brain, the right mind, and psychoanalisys. University of California at Los Angeles School of Medicine, Los Angeles

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegal M, Varle R (2002) Neural systems involved in ‘theory of mind’. Nat Rev Neurosci 3:463–471

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Siegel DJ (1999) The developing mind: toward a neurobiology of interpersonal experience. Guilford Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith EE, Kosslyn SM (2007) Cognitive psychology: mind and brain. Prentice Hall, Upper Saddle River

    Google Scholar 

  • Solms M (1996) Towards an anatomy of the unconscious. J Clin Psychoanal 5:331–367

    Google Scholar 

  • Spitz R (1965) The first year of life: a psychoanalytic study of normal and deviant development of object relations. International Universities Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Springer SP, Deutsch G (1998) left brain, right brain perspective from cognitive neuroscience, 5th edn. WH Freeman and Company, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Sroufe LA (1995) Emotional development. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Stern DN (1985) The interpersonal world of the infant. Basic Books, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Stone WL, Lee EB, Ashford L, Brissie J, Hepburn SL, Coonrod EE, Wess BH (1999) Can autism be diagnosed accurately in children under 3 years? J Child Psychol Psychiatr 40(2):219–226

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Stuss DT, GGJr Gallup, Alexander MP (2001) The frontal lobes are necessary for ‘theory of mind’. Brain 124:279–286

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thatcher RW, Walker RA, Guidice S (1987) Human cerebral hemispheres develop at different rates and ages. Science 236:110–1113

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen C (1979) Communication and cooperation in early infancy: a description of primary intersubjectivity. In: Bullowa M (ed) Before speech: the beginnings of human communication. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 321–347

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen C (1998) The concept and foundations of infant intersubjectivity. In: Braten S (ed) Intersubjective communication and emotion in early ontogeny. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 15–46

    Google Scholar 

  • Tucker DM (1981) Lateral brain function, emotion, and conceptualization. Psychol Bull Jan 89(1):19–46

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Tucker DM (1992) Developing emotions and cortical networks. In: Gunnar M, Nelson C (eds) Minnesota symposium on child psychology. Developmental behavioral neuroscience, vol 24. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, pp 75–128

    Google Scholar 

  • Varela FJ, Thompson E, Rosch E (1991) The embodied mind: cognitive science and human experience. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Varela FJ, Lachaux JP, Rodriguez E, Martinerie J (2001) The brainweb: phase synchronization and large-scale integration. Nat Rev Neurosci 2(4):229–239

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Watson JS (1994) Detection of self: the perfect algorithm. In: Parker ST, Mitchell RW, Boccia ML (eds) Self-awareness in animals and humans: developmental perspectives. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 131–148

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiss KM (1990) Duplication with variation: metameric logic in evolution from genes to morphology. Am J Phys Anthropol 33:1–23

    Google Scholar 

  • Willshaw DJ, Von Der Malsburg C (1976) How patterned neural connections can be set up by self-organization. Proc Roy Soc Lond Ser B 194:431–445

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Winnicott D (1953) Transitional objects and transitional phenomena. Int J Psychoanal 34:89–97

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Winnicott D (1960) Ego distortion in terms of the True and False Self. In: Winnicott D (ed) The maturational process and the facilitating environment. Hogarth Press, London, pp 140–152

  • Wolff PH (1968) Sucking patterns of infant mammals. Brain Behav Evol 1:354–367

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Giampaolo Sasso.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Sasso, G. Dynamic sensory–motor oscillation and cerebral development. Cogn Process 11, 307–329 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-009-0346-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-009-0346-z

Keywords

Navigation