Skip to main content
Log in

The verbalization of multiple strategies in a variant of the traveling salesperson problem

  • Research Report
  • Published:
Cognitive Processing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

What kinds of strategies do humans employ when confronted with a complex spatial task, and how do they verbalize these strategies? Previous research concerned with the well-known traveling salesperson problem (TSP) typically aimed at the identification of a generally applicable heuristics that adequately represents human behavior in relation to the abstract task of combining points. This paper adopts a novel perspective in two respects. On the one hand, it addresses the strategies people employ when confronted with a more complex task, involving distractors and feature information rather than identical points. On the other hand, retrospective linguistic representations of the strategies used are analyzed in relation to the behavioral data, using discourse analytic methods. Results show that both the behavioral results and the verbalizations point to a range of strategies related to those proposed for solving abstract TSPs. However, in contrast to earlier accounts in the literature, the participants employ a repertory of multi-faceted strategies and planning processes, simplifying and structuring the problem space across subtasks and processes in flexible ways. These findings provide further insight into the nature of human strategies in spatial problem solving tasks and their retrospective verbalization, highlighting how procedures generally known in the literature may be adapted to more complex tasks, and how they may be verbalized spontaneously.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6

References

  • Allen GL (1999) Spatial abilities, cognitive maps, and wayfinding: bases for individual differences in spatial cognition and behavior. In: Golledge R (ed) Wayfinding behavior: cognitive maps and other spatial processes. Johns Hopkins University Press, Baltimore, pp 46–80

    Google Scholar 

  • Applegate DL, Bixby RE, Chvátal V, Cook WJ (2007) The traveling salesman problem: a computational study. Princeton University Press, NJ

  • Basso D, Bisiacchi PS, Cotelli M, Farinello C (2001) Planning times during traveling salesman’s problem: differences between closed head injury and normal subjects. Brain Cogn 46(1–2):38–42. doi:10.1016/S0278-2626(01)80029-4

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Best BJ, Simon HA (2000) Simulating human performance on the traveling salesman problem. In: Taatgen N, Aasman J (eds) Proceedings of the third international conference on cognitive modeling. Universal Press, Groningen, pp 42–49

    Google Scholar 

  • Couclelis H (1996) Verbal directions for way-finding: space, cognition, and language. In: Portugali J (ed) The construction of cognitive maps. Kluwer, Dordrecht, pp 133–153

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Dascal M (1983) Pragmatics and the philosophy of mind 1: thought in language. John Benjamins, Amsterdam

    Google Scholar 

  • Denis M, Pazzaglia F, Cornoldi C, Bertolo L (1999) Spatial discourse and navigation: an analysis of route directions in the city of Venice. Appl Cogn Psychol 13(2):145–174. doi :10.1002/(SICI)1099-0720(199904)13:2<145::AID-ACP550>3.0.CO;2-4

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dry M, Lee MD, Vickers D, Hughes P (2006) Human performance on visually presented traveling salesperson problems with varying numbers of nodes. J Probl Solving 1(1):20–32

    Google Scholar 

  • Elliot AJ, Maier MA (2007) Color and psychological functioning. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 16:250–254. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00514.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson KA, Simon HA (1984) Protocol analysis: verbal reports as data. Bradford books/MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Gärling T (1989) The role of cognitive maps in spatial decisions. J Environ Psychol 9:269–278. doi:10.1016/S0272-4944(89)80009-X

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gärling T, Säisä J, Böök A, Lindberg E (1986) The spatiotemporal sequencing of everyday activities in the large-scale environment. J Environ Psychol 6:261–280. doi:10.1016/S0272-4944(86)80001-9

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Golden B, Bodin L, Doyle T, Stewart W Jr (1980) Approximate traveling salesman algorithms. Oper Res 28(3:2):694–711

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Golledge RG (1997) Defining the criteria used in path selection. In: Ettema DF, Timmermans HJP (eds) Activity-based approaches to travel analysis. Elsevier, New York, pp 151–169

    Google Scholar 

  • Golledge RG (ed) (1999) Wayfinding behavior: cognitive mapping and other spatial processes. Johns Hopkins University Press, Baltimore

    Google Scholar 

  • Graham SM, Joshi A, Pizlo Z (2000) The traveling salesman problem: a hierarchical model. Mem Cognit 28:1191–1204

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Halliday MAK (1994) An introduction to functional grammar, 2nd edn edn. Edward Arnold, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes-Roth B, Hayes-Roth F (1979) A cognitive model of planning. Cogn Sci 3:275–310. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog0304_1

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Helfenstein S, Saariluoma P (2007) Apperception in primed problem solving. Cogn Process 8:211–232. doi:10.1007/s10339-007-0189-4

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Janzen G, Weststeijn CG (2007) Neural representation of object location and route direction: an event-related fMRI study. Brain Res 1165:116–125. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2007.05.074

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kim MS, Cave KR (1999) Grouping effects on spatial attention in visual search. J Gen Psychol 126:326–352

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Liqiang QH, Holcombe AO, Pashler H (2004) Repetition priming in visual search: episodic retrieval, not feature priming. Mem Cognit 32(1):12–20

    Google Scholar 

  • Loomis J, Klatzky R, Golledge R, Philbeck J (1999) Human navigation by path integration. In: Golledge R (ed) Wayfinding behavior: cognitive mapping and other spatial processes. Johns Hopkins University Press, Baltimore, pp 125–152

    Google Scholar 

  • MacGregor JN, Ormerod TC (1996) Human performance on the travelling salesman problem. Percept Psychophys 58:527–539

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • MacGregor JN, Ormerod TC, Chronicle E (2000) A model of human performance on the travelling salesperson problem. Mem Cognit 7:1183–1190

    Google Scholar 

  • Payne JW, Bettman JR, Coupey E, Johnson EJ (1992) A constructive process view of decision making: multiple strategies in judgment and choice. Acta Psychol (Amst) 80:107–141. doi:10.1016/0001-6918(92)90043-D

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Polivanova NI (1974) On some functional and structural features of the visual-intuitive components of a problem-solving process. Vopr Psychologii 4:41–51

    Google Scholar 

  • Reineking T, Kohlhagen C, Zetzsche C (2008) Efficient wayfinding in hierarchically regionalized spatial environments. In: Proceedings of Spatial Cognition, Sep. 15–19, Freiburg

  • van Rooij I, Stege U, Schactman A (2003) Convex hull and tour crossings in the Euclidean traveling salesperson problem: implications for human performance studies. Mem Cognit 31(2):215–220

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tenbrink T (2008) The verbalization of cognitive processes: thinking-aloud data and retrospective reports. In: Ramm W, Fabricius-Hansen C (eds) Linearisation and segmentation in discourse. Multidisciplinary approaches to discourse 2008 (MAD 08) Feb 20–23, Univ. of Oslo, Lysebu, Oslo, pp 125–135

  • Tenbrink T, Wiener J (2007) Wayfinding strategies in behavior and language: a symmetric and interdisciplinary approach to cognitive processes. In: Barkowsky T, Knauff M, Ligozat G, Montello D (eds) Spatial cognition V: reasoning, action, interaction. Springer, Berlin, pp 401–420

    Google Scholar 

  • Todd PM, Gigerenzer G (2000) Précis of simple heuristics that make us smart. Behav Brain Sci 23:727–780. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00003447

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Vickers D, Butavicius M, Lee MD, Medvedev A (2001) Human performance on visually presented traveling salesman problems. Psychol Res 65:34–45. doi:10.1007/s004260000031

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Vickers D, Lee MD, Dry M, Hughes P (2003) The roles of the convex hull and the number of potential intersections in performance on visually presented traveling salesperson problems. Mem Cognit 31(7):1094–1104

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wiener JM, Tenbrink T (2008) Traveling salesman problem: The human case. Künstliche Intelligenz. KI und Kognition 1(08):18–22

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiener JM, Schnee A, Mallot HA (2004) Use and interaction of navigation strategies in regionalized environments. J Environ Psychol 24(4):475–493

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiener JM, Tenbrink T, Henschel J, Hölscher C (2008a) Situated and prospective path planning: route choice in an urban environment. In: CogSci 2008: 30th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, July 23–26, Washington

  • Wiener JM, Ehbauer NN, Mallot HA (2008b) Planning paths to multiple targets: memory involvement and planning strategies (under review)

  • Williams CC, Henderson JM, Zacks RT (2005) Incidental visual memory for targets and distractors in visual search. Percept Psychophys 67:816–827

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Funding by the Volkswagen Foundation is gratefully acknowledged. We thank Elena Andonova, Lucie Salwiczek, and Inessa Seifert for helpful comments on an earlier version of this paper, our student assistants and the participants for their valuable contributions to this project, and the anonymous reviewers for providing excellent advice. The experiments comply with the current laws in Germany. All participants gave their informed consent prior to the participation in the study, and all procedures administered complied with the ethical guidelines of the German Psychological society (DGPs).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Thora Tenbrink.

Appendices

Appendix

Example texts by one participant (Exp. 2: regionalized version)

Text 1: (Describe in as much detail as possible how you have solved the task)

“Zuerst habe ich mir die Zeichen in einer beliebigen Reihenfolge auf dem Feld angeschaut und “geistig” miteinander verbunden, dann habe ich versucht herauszufinden, an welchen Stellen ich den Weg verkürzen könnte. Dabei war es meist sinnvoller, die Zeichen der gleichen Farbe nacheinander abzuhaken, weil das eigene Farbfeld meist näher liegt als ein Anderes. Oft habe ich festgestellt, dass die Zeichen, die ich als zweiten oder dritten Schritt gewählt hatte sich viel besser am Schluss eingliederten->darin sah ich auch eine der Hauptkomplikationen. Eine Andere ist das schwarze Quadrat, da es meist als Mitte fungierte und somit die Punkte größer über das Blatt verteilt waren”.

Table 2 Strategy distribution in both experiments according to participants
Table 3 References to the path as trajectory: nouns (absolute numbers of occurrences)
Table 4 Features of the path: attributes (absolute numbers of occurrences)
Table 5 Processes associated with the path: verbs (absolute numbers of occurrences)

First I looked at the symbols in a random order in the field and “mentally” connected them with each other, then I tried to find out at which places I could shorten the path. In doing this it was mostly more sensible to tick off the symbols of the same color one after another, because the same color region was mostly closer than another. Often I realized that the symbols that I chose as second or third step were much better suitable for the end->that’s also where I saw one of the main complications. Another is the black square, since it typically served as midpoint and therefore the symbols were distributed more widely across the field.

Text 2: (Please write an instruction for a good friend of yours that allows him/her to solve the task as well as possible)

“Überlege dir, wie der kürzeste Weg über die auf der Liste aufgeführten Symbole aussehen könnte. Die Ausgangssymbol ist mit “Start” gekennzeichnet und ist gleichzeitig das Endsymbol. Achte vor allem auf das Ende der Strecke und überprüfe nochmals deine ersten Punkte. Wäre es vielleicht sinnvoller der Punkt am Ende einzugliedern? Denke nochmals nach, bevor du die Punkte legst. Wenn du einen Fehler gemacht hast, versuche ihn zu analysieren um ihn das nächste Mal zu vermeiden. Oft wird der kürzeste Weg erst offensichtlich, wenn die Punkte liegen, da du sie nicht verschieben darfst, versuche sie dir vorzustellen. Notiere anschließend die Zahlen vom zweiten Blatt (also die Positionen auf denen die Punkte liegen).”

Consider how the shortest path across the symbols shown in the list could look like. The starting symbol is marked as “start” and is simultaneously the end symbol. Pay particular attention to the end of the path and check again your first symbols. Could it be more sensible to integrate the symbol at the end? Reconsider again before you place the markers. If you have made a mistake, try to analyze it in order to avoid it next time. Often the shortest path only becomes obvious when the markers are placed, because you are not allowed to move them, try to imagine them. Note subsequently the numbers from the second sheet (that is, the positions on which the symbols lie).

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Tenbrink, T., Wiener, J. The verbalization of multiple strategies in a variant of the traveling salesperson problem. Cogn Process 10, 143–161 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-008-0225-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-008-0225-z

Keywords

Navigation