Behavioral studies and veterinary management of orangutans at Bukit Merah Orang Utan Island, Perak, Malaysia

Abstract

The Bukit Merah Orang Utan Island (OUI) Foundation has been conducting behavioral and veterinary research on orangutans as an attempt at ex situ conservation. Since 2010, the Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University has been collaborating with OUI to promote environmental enrichment and infant rearing by biological mothers in addition to the continuous efforts of refining the veterinary management of the endangered species. In 2011, three Bornean orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus pygmaeus) were released on an island, called BJ Island, adjacent to OUI. This island is approximately 5.6 ha in size, and 635 trees belonging to 102 plant species were identified prior to their release. Behavioral monitoring of the released individuals has been conducted to evaluate their behavioral adaptation to the new environment. Two of the three released orangutans were born in the wild, whereas the youngest individual was born on OUI and expected to learn forest survival strategies from the two older individuals. One of the orangutans was pregnant at the time of release and subsequently gave birth to two male infants on BJ Island. The behavioral monitoring indicated that these orangutans traveled more and spent more time on trees following their release onto BJ Island. However, resting was longer for two females both on OUI and BJ Island when compared to other populations. The orangutans consumed some natural food resources on BJ Island. The release into a more naturalistic environment may help the orangutans to develop more naturalistic behavioral patterns that resemble their wild counterparts.

Introduction

Orangutans are the only species of great ape found in only two Asian countries, namely Malaysia and Indonesia (Delgado and van Schaik 2000; Husson et al. 2009). In 2016, the Bornean orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus) was categorized as a critically endangered species in the International Union for Conservation of Nature Red List (Ancrenaz et al. 2016). To cease the decline in the number of orangutans, effective in situ and ex situ conservation and management techniques need to be developed and implemented through international collaborations. Compared to the African great apes that live in social groups comprising multiple adult individuals, orangutans have a relatively solitary lifestyle, with only mothers and their infants remaining in constant physical proximity and contact, although the existence of loose social organization and regional variation has been suggested (Russon 2009a; van Schaik et al. 2009). Considerable number of orangutan rehabilitation and re-introduction efforts has been reported from 12 projects (eight of which were active in 2009: six in Borneo and two in Sumatra), and effective protocols, as well as practical constraints have been suggested in these sites ranging within orangutans’ natural habitat (Russon 2002, 2009b; Grundmann 2006; Descovich et al. 2011; Kuze et al. 2008, 2012; Robins et al. 2013).

In Malaysia, part of Borneo is home to two subspecies of orangutans: P. pygmaeus pygmaeus and P. pygmaeus morio. Wild orangutans have already disappeared from Peninsular Malaysia, but fossil records of orangutans have been found there (Tshen 2016). Ibrahim et al. (2013) reported that orangutans persisted in Peninsular Malaysia around 33–66 thousand years ago, but declined in number because of climate change and paleogeographical constraints that prevented the reconstruction of local populations even after the climatic amelioration in the Late Pleistocene.

Orang Utan Island (OUI), which is located in Perak, Peninsular Malaysia, was established in February 2000 with assistance from the Bukit Merah Laketown Project and EMKAY Group (http://emkay.com.my/). The early operations are reported in Agoramoorthy (2002). OUI currently houses 22 orangutan individuals, the family lineage of which is shown in Table 1. Eleven orangutans (P. pygmaeus) were initially introduced from Sarawak Forestry and Malacca Zoo, Malaysia, between 2000 and 2002, four of which have died. Since then, 21 orangutans have been born on OUI and successfully lived there for more than 2 years, indicating the reproductive success of this endangered species, which is considered rather difficult to breed in captive settings of some countries without a strategic breeding program such as the Species Survival Plan operated in North America region (Cocks 2007). Initially on OUI, if an infant had health problems, it was separated from its mother to provide intensive veterinary treatments and care by humans for 24 h per day. This practice enabled the accumulation of veterinary knowledge on orangutan development, as well as protocols for veterinary care and managements, behavioral rehabilitation, and introduction to an enclosure with other conspecifics (Dharmalingam et al. 2012; Sabapathy 2013).

Table 1 Family lineage of Orang Utan Island (OUI) orangutans. Red line shows the period of mother-rearing

In 2008, the Bukit Merah Orang Utan Island Foundation was established, which aimed to promote an effort for the ex situ conservation of orangutan’s population. OUI receives many visitors (42,747 in 2015), school visits for education programs (2,823 students in 2015), researchers, and internship students (such as from Universiti Sains Malaysia as reported by Mashhor and Anuar 2013). Thus, OUI functions as a window for environmental education on the peninsular side of Malaysia, and attracts both local and international visitors.

In 2010, the Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University, started collaboration with OUI. We shifted the key focus to the promotion of environmental enrichment and infant rearing by the biological mother, two essential standards for keeping great apes (McCann et al. 2007). Mother rearing has been shown to be particularly important for infants’ cognitive and social development in great apes (Hayashi and Matsuzawa 2017). African great apes live in social groups, and so their offspring are able to stay near to the mother in the natal group for at least several years after weaning. Orangutan offspring have the longest dependent period of approximately 7–8 years (van Noordwijk and van Schaik 2005); however, they need to be independent from the mother at the time of weaning because she may have difficulty accompanying two dependent offsprings at a time (van Adrichem et al. 2006; Mendonça et al. 2017). Since orangutans live in an environment where the amount of natural food resources always fluctuates (Kanamori et al. 2010, 2017), long-term postnatal learning from the mother about the site-specific environment and its variation through time is essential for the infant’s survival in the solitary life after weaning.

In 2011, three orangutans were released on BJ Island, which is adjacent to OUI, as the final stage of the behavioral rehabilitation program at OUI and the first step toward realizing the final goal of wild release. The rescued infant orangutans are supposed to undergo a rehabilitation program in a rehabilitation center located in their natural habitat, allowing them to be gradually habituated to forest living from infancy. In contrast, we released three orangutan individuals from juvenile and adult age categories. The present paper reports on the current situation of behavioral studies and veterinary management on OUI together with the results of behavioral monitoring of the ex-captive orangutans in an island of Peninsular Malaysia.

Methods

Subjects

OUI currently holds 22 orangutan individuals (Table 1). Three individuals kept on OUI were selected to be released on BJ Island on February 15, 2011. This included a subadult male Ah Ling, 14 years old at the time of release, an adult female Nickey, 19 years old, and a juvenile female Sonia, 8 years old.

The adult female Nickey was pregnant at the time of release and subsequently gave birth to a male offspring, William, in March 2011. Unfortunately, William died from respiratory illness in 2013. Nickey also gave birth to a second male offspring, Chacha, on BJ Island in September 2014.

Facilities

OUI currently operates five outdoor enclosures ranging from around 1000 to 2400 m2 that are separated by electronic hot-wire fences (Fig. 1). Each enclosure is covered by natural vegetation, and contains ropes connecting the trees, wooden platforms, and feeding devices. The orangutans have daily access to the outdoor enclosures from 9 am to 5 pm. During the night, they return to their night cages, where they are kept individually or with some cage mates.

Fig. 1
figure1

Overview of Orang Utan Island (OUI) and BJ Island

Human visitors to OUI walk through the tunnel that is located in the middle of the orangutan enclosures, from which they can observe the orangutans moving freely in the enclosures through metal frames covering the tunnel. At the beginning of the collaboration with the Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University, OUI connected high trees in the orangutan enclosures by increasing the number of ropes to facilitate arboreal transfer between trees. In addition, some materials were attached to the cages in the holding area for feeding enrichment. The outdoor enclosures and the visitors’ tunnel were expanded in 2012, with the latter currently being approximately 200 m long, enabling the observation of orangutans in the enlarged outdoor-enclosures that contain a lot of natural vegetation and trees.

From 2010, mothers were encouraged to take care of their own infants for as long as possible. Most of the mothers had no difficulty holding their infants from the beginning and the skill of smooth breast feeding developed gradually through mother–infant interactions. In the case of orangutan mothers who had a good relationship with human caretakers, humans were able to give medication to the infants, and control both the mothers and infants in a direct keeping-style.

Only the infants who lost their mothers have been hand-reared by humans since 2010. Prior to this, the orangutan infants were closely cared for by humans at the Infant Care Unit (ICU) for 24 h per day until they were approximately 1 year of age. Infants of around 1–4 years of age were then kept as a group in the Enrichment Development Unit cage, which was provided with enrichment materials such as ropes, hammocks, and natural tree branches in order to develop their locomotor skills, during which time they spontaneously developed the social skills to interact and play with other individuals which are kept in the same group. After this stage, they were transferred to an outdoor enclosure to develop tree climbing skills, where they maintained their social interactions with conspecifics including adolescent and adult individuals.

During the final phase of hand rearing by humans, some behavioral studies were also conducted. The first author went into the same cage as the infant orangutans to conduct cognitive developmental tests that were originally developed to test and compare African great apes and human infants (Hayashi and Matsuzawa 2003; Hayashi 2007). Currently, no infants are kept in ICU and the Enrichment Development Unit, indicating the fundamental success of the rearing attempts by OUI orangutan mothers.

BJ Island is 1.1 km away from OUI and approximately 5.6 ha in size. Researchers and students from Pulau Banding Rainforest Research Centre, Universiti Sains Malaysia (USM), and OUI staff surveyed the vegetation on BJ Island before releasing the orangutans and identified 635 trees belonging to 102 plant species of 70 genera, 35 families (Supplementary Table). Prior to the release, we created a small human visiting area on the island that included a jetty, researchers’ hut, generator for the hot-wire fence dividing the two areas, and orangutan cage for quarantine purposes.

Data collection and statistics

Health conditions of orangutans on OUI are closely monitored by veterinary staff. The procedure of health monitoring for orangutans, as well as the protocol for diagnosis and treatment has been established on OUI (Fig. 2). Previous studies have reported the veterinary management of orangutans in the captive setting of OUI (Dharmalingam 2015a, b, 2016).

Fig. 2
figure2

Flowchart of health monitoring procedure for orangutans in Orang Utan Island (OUI)

Three orangutans from OUI were anesthetized, individually placed in transfer cages, and transferred to BJ Island, where they were released on February 15, 2011. Behavioral monitoring was conducted before and after the release of these orangutans using focal animal sampling at 1-min intervals (Martin and Bateson 2007). The present analysis used the behavioral data collected by local staff before (December 2010 and January 2011) and after (June, August, and September 2011) the release. Supplemental feeding has been conducted twice per day to compensate for the scarcity of natural fruiting trees on BJ Island. Minimal human contact has been maintained with the orangutans on BJ Island, with only the core keepers and research staff entering the orangutan area to feed and observe them.

We applied Pearson’s Chi-squared test for each individual by using R version 3.4.2 (2017 The R Foundation for Statistical Computing) to compare orangutans’ behavior on OUI and BJ Island. In order to apply the Chi-square test, five infrequent behavioral-categories (Social/Object Play, Social/Self Groom, and Others) were pooled into one category of “Miscellaneous”, although the percentage of each behavioral category is shown in Table 3. Exact number of recorded data-points in four behavioral categories (Feed, Rest, Travel, and Miscellaneous) were summed up in two sites (OUI and BJ Island) for each individual and used for the analysis. When the results were statistically significant, a residual analysis was conducted to detect, which behavioral category yielded the significant difference among the two sites (p < 0.05) by using a R-program provided by js-STAR. Exact number of recorded data-points for the stay on ground or arboreal were summed up in two sites for each individual and used for a Pearson’s Chi-squared test with Yates’ continuity corrections.

Results

Orangutans sometimes developed symptoms and received veterinary care and treatment on OUI. Common diseases occurring on OUI are listed in Table 2. Top two diseases of Strongyloidiasis and Balantadidiasis were also common in other captive/rehabilitation centers and wild sites (Labes et al. 2010, Foitová et al. 2009). Amoebic dysentery caused by Entamoeba sp. and Ascariasis were less prevalent, but within the repertoire in seven and eight out of 10 populations, respectively. Although we lack comparable data for some diseases caused by bacteria, some diseases may be caused by the close proximity among conspecifics and with human visitors in the captive setting.

Table 2 Common diseases occur in Orang Utan Island (OUI). The frequency of occurrence was shown as the percentage of orangutan individuals infected by each disease per year. The right three columns show the comparable data on parasite infection from two previous studies

In the outdoor enclosures of OUI, the orangutans used the natural vegetation (Fig. 3) and ropes for locomotion, manipulative play, and making day nests. Play behavior was frequently observed between multiple individuals in peer groups of similarly aged young orangutans. Although the orangutans occasionally remained on the ground to play or interact with peers and human caretakers, they also climbed the trees for both playing and resting. Many interesting behavioral patterns that allow us to make inferences about the orangutans’ cognitive development were observed on OUI, including hitting or digging actions using an object in hand, the use of tools to touch and open the hot-wire fence, and social interactions with conspecifics and free-ranging crab-eating macaques (Macaca fascicularis).

Fig. 3
figure3

Orangutans in an outdoor enclosure in Orang Utan Island (OUI)

A behavioral comparison of the three orangutans that were released on BJ Island with those on OUI revealed several patterns (Table 3). Activity budgets of three orangutans were significantly different between OUI and BJ Island (Ah Ling: χ2 = 120, df = 3, p < 0.0001, Nickey: χ2 = 20, df = 3, p = 0.0002, Sonia: χ2 = 150, df = 3, p < 0.0001). The residual analysis revealed significant differences (p < 0.05) as listed in below (Ah Ling: Rest and Travel was longer on BJ Island and Miscellaneous was longer on OUI, Nickey: Travel and Miscellaneous was longer on BJ Island and Rest was longer on OUI, Sonia: Feed and Travel was longer on BJ Island and Miscellaneous was longer on OUI). Thus, the consistent pattern among the three orangutans was prolonged traveling on BJ Island. Comparison to the previous report from four wild (three in Borneo and one in Sumatra) and four rehabilitant orangutans (three in Borneo and one in Sumatra); in total, eight populations (Russon 2009b) revealed that some behaviors were out of the range of activity budgets in previous reports. Resting was longer in two females both on OUI and BJ Island (62.1 and 54.7% for Nickey and 55.7 and 53.8% for Sonia) than the normal range in the wild (10.9–50.6%) and in rehabilitant (13.9–49.6%). On OUI, feeding for Sonia (10.4%) and traveling for Nickey (9.3%) were shorter than the normal range in the wild (31.9–69.3% for feeding and 12.2–18.2% for traveling) and in rehabilitant (20.8–64.1% for feeding and 10.8–18.7% for traveling). On BJ Island, traveling for Ah Ling (20.5%) and Sonia (21.0%) was slightly above the normal range in the wild (12.2–18.2%) and in rehabilitant (10.8–18.7%).

Table 3 Activity budgets for basic behavioral patterns in each orangutan on Orang Utan Island (OUI) and BJ Island

Positional behavior of three orangutans (stay on ground vs arboreal stay) were significantly different between OUI and BJ Island (Ah Ling: χ2 = 94, df = 1, p < 0.0001, Nickey: χ2 = 190, df = 1, p < 0.0001, Sonia: χ2 = 55, df = 1, p < 0.0001). All subjects stayed longer on trees rather than on ground on BJ Island.

The orangutans that were transferred to BJ Island successfully made a nest from the first night. They also occasionally consumed natural vegetation on the island (see the detail in Supplementary Table) in addition to the supplemental fruits and vegetables provided by humans. Orangutans consumed 14 species (14 genera of 10 families) from natural vegetation on BJ Island: fruits of two species, leaves of 11 species, bark of three species, stems of one species, and shoots of one species. The youngest individual, Sonia, consumed more food items (13 items) compared to older individuals (Ah Ling: seven items, Nickey: five items). However, comparison with previous reports from 11 wild (eight in Borneo and three in Sumatra) and four rehabilitant (three in Borneo and one in Sumatra) populations (Galdikas 1988; Kanamori et al. 2010; Russon et al. 2007; Russon et al. 2009) showed that only 10 species were consumed by orangutans of BJ Island, as well as the other sites. Four species were consumed only by orangutans of BJ Island and only leaves and stems were consumed in this category. Although 78 species were present on BJ Island and were recorded as food items in other sites, orangutans on BJ Island did not consumed these plant species. Remaining 10 species were present on BJ Island, but orangutans on BJ Island nor other sites do not consume these plants. The orangutans occasionally encountered and interacted with each other in the forest or near the human area. The youngest individual also used natural vegetation for object play (Fig. 4).

Fig. 4
figure4

The youngest orangutan on BJ Island (Sonia) playing with a large fallen leaf

The mother, Nickey, had four previous births before giving birth to her son, William, on BJ Island. She experienced initial difficulty in breast feeding the baby smoothly as she had no previous experience in taking care of her offspring for an extended period of time. For the first few weeks, the baby emitted whimpers when he needed breast feeding, but failed to contact the mother’s nipple on his own. Through these mother–infant interactions, the mother gradually learned appropriate maternal behaviors, including breast feeding. Intensive mother–infant interactions including play behaviors were observed throughout the study period and food sharing frequently occurred between the mother and infant after the soliciting behaviors from the infant (Emi Yamamoto, personal communication; Fig. 5). Although the first infant born on BJ Island (William) died from a respiratory disease, the mother succeeded in rearing her own offspring under the naturalistic conditions of the forest for more than 2 years. Moreover, she gave birth to her next offspring on BJ Island 1 year and 1 month after the death of William.

Fig. 5
figure5

Infant orangutan (William, 1 year 6 months old) eating a sugarcane from the hand of his mother (Nickey)

Discussion

Activity budget of three orangutans were significantly different on OUI and BJ Island. More specifically, time spent for traveling and arboreal stay was significantly longer on BJ Island constantly across all three orangutans. In the subadult male, the social interaction with conspecifics was decreased after the release on BJ Island. These results may indicate that the more naturalistic environment yielded the emergence of more appropriate behavioral patterns which resemble to those of wild counterparts. In comparison to the range of activity budgets reported from other orangutan populations, resting time for two females was longer both on OUI and BJ Islands. Feeding of younger female and traveling of adult female was shorter on OUI compared to other populations. Time for traveling was slightly above the normal range for the male and younger female on BJ Island. These results may indicate that BJ Island was a more suitable environment for eliciting more naturalistic behavior in orangutans. However, even on BJ Island, the resting time of females was above the range in other populations, indicating that we need more effort in achieving the activity budget, which is closer to the wild orangutans (Morrogh-Bernard et al. 2009).

Unfortunately, it was necessary to provide the orangutans on BJ Island with supplemental foods due to the scarcity of fruiting trees here. Activity budget for feeding was less and resting was more than the normal range in some of the orangutans on BJ Island. This result can be explained by the provisioning of nutritious fruits and vegetables which decreased the necessity of foraging in the forest. Thus, the number of natural food items consumed by orangutans on BJ Island (14 species) was very low compared to the wild orangutans’ food repertoire (Leighton 1993) and we found only 10 species were consumed by both parties. Therefore, if we think about the next step of wild release, we need to further improve the foraging skills of the orangutans by increasing food repertoire to the species present in Peninsular Malaysia and within the food repertoire of wild orangutans (78 species on BJ Island).

As these orangutans were kept in human proximity for many years, it may be difficult to keep them at an appropriate distance from humans, making behavioral monitoring difficult as suggested by a previous study (Smith 2009). The present paper reports on a rare case of rehabilitation effort of individuals including adult, whereby the subadult male, Ah Ling, developed his cheek pad during the behavioral monitoring on BJ Island and became more aggressive toward humans. The youngest orangutan had no fear of artificial objects and so broke the trap camera after exploration. This sort of habituation is more typical and makes it difficult to monitor behavior using trap cameras.

OUI currently promotes mother rearing to support natural development in infant orangutans. Although some mothers on OUI and BJ Island had initial difficulties with breast feeding, they never rejected holding their infants. Younger individuals carefully observed mothers taking care of their infants and sometimes interacted with the infants. These hands-on experiences may lead to the development of normal maternal behavior in the young females who were born on OUI and were not reared by their biological mothers.

Although wild orangutans do not naturally occur in Peninsular Malaysia, this region was historically a part of their range, making it easier to conduct ex situ conservation. As a zoo environment, OUI can provide an ideal environment for orangutans in terms of ambient temperature, basic vegetation, and local fruits as they are very close to their wild habitat. In addition, visitors to OUI are able to see the orangutans climbing trees, and this direct experience may lead them to think about their natural habitat and orangutan conservation in the wild habitats. Thus, OUI is a suitable place for conducting environmental education, as well as behavioral and veterinary research on orangutans.

The common diseases occurred on OUI were also reported from other facilities and sites. The establishment and refining of standard protocol for treatment of common diseases in captive environment is one of the essential issues for promoting orangutan conservation. Although human contact was minimized on BJ Island, some problems occurred during the rehabilitation process. Since the orangutans had spent a long time in human proximity, it was hard to maintain an appropriate distance between humans and the orangutans during behavioral monitoring. This may explain the cause of death of the first infant born on BJ Island (William), who may have contracted a respiratory disease from a human observer. Similarly, Kuze et al. (2012) indicated that the risk of infant mortality is higher for rehabilitated orangutan mothers. However, it is important to continue these conservation and rehabilitation efforts for endangered orangutans. The careful establishment of an appropriate protocol for veterinary managements and behavioral rehabilitation of ex-captive orangutans adapting into semi-wild environments is needed, and these attempts should be continuously monitored and evaluated through behavioral observation.

References

  1. Agoramoorthy G (2002) Exhibiting orang-utans on a natural island in Malaysia. Int Zoo News 49:260–266

    Google Scholar 

  2. Ancrenaz M, Gumal M, Marshall AJ, Meijaard E, Wich SA, Husson S (2016) Pongo pygmaeus. The IUCN Red List of Threatened Species 2016: e.T17975A17966347

  3. Cocks L (2007) Factors affecting mortality, fertility, and well-being in relations to species differences in captive orangutans. Int J Primatol 28:421–428

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Delgado RA, van Schaik CP (2000) The behavioral ecology and conservation of the orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus): a tale of two islands. Evol Anthropol 9:201–218

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Descovich KA, Galdikas BM, Tribe A, Lisle A, Phillips CJ (2011) Fostering appropriate behavior in rehabilitant orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus). Int J Primatol 32:616–633

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Dharmalingam S (2015a) Temperature management in infant orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus) at Orang Utan Island, Bukit Merah, Perak, Malaysia. Merit Res J Med Med Sci 3:497–501

    Google Scholar 

  7. Dharmalingam S (2015b) Meningitis in infant orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus) at Orang Utan Island, Bukit Merah, Perak, Malaysia. Merit Res J Med Med Sci 3:555–564

    Google Scholar 

  8. Dharmalingam S (2016) Respiratory tract infection in infant orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus) at Orang Utan Island, Bukit Merah, Perak, Malaysia. Pyrex J Med Med Sci 3:5–9

    Google Scholar 

  9. Dharmalingam S, Hapiszudin N, Roslan R (2012) The orangutans of Bukit Merah. Bukit Merah Orang Utan Island Foundation, Bukit Merah

    Google Scholar 

  10. Foitová I, Huffman MA, Wisnu N, Olšanský M (2009) Parasites and their impacts on orangutan health. In: Wich SA, Atmoko SU, Setia TM, van Schaik CP (eds) Orangutans: geographic variation in behavioral ecology and conservation. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 157–169

    Google Scholar 

  11. Galdikas BMF (1988) Orangutan diet, range, and activity at Tanjung Putting, Central Borneo. Int J Primatol 9:1–35

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Grundmann E (2006) Back to the wild: will reintroduction and rehabilitation help the long-term conservation of orang-utans in Indonesia? Soc Sci Info 45:265–284

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Hayashi M (2007) Stacking of blocks by chimpanzees: developmental process and physical understanding. Anim Cogn 10:89–103

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Hayashi M, Matsuzawa T (2003) Cognitive development in object manipulation by infant chimpanzees. Anim Cogn 6:225–233

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Hayashi M, Matsuzawa T (2017) Mother–infant interaction in captive and wild chimpanzees. Inf Behav Develop 48:20–29

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Husson SJ, Wich SA, Marshall AJ, Dennis RD, Ancrenaz M, Brassey R, Gumal M, Hearn AJ, Meijaard E, Simorangkir T, Singleton I (2009) Orangutan distribution, density, abundance and impacts of disturbance. In: Wich SA, Atmoko SU, Setia TM, van Schaik CP (eds) Orangutans: geographic variation in behavioral ecology and conservation. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 77–96

    Google Scholar 

  17. Ibrahim YKh, Tshen LT, Westaway KE, Cranbrook E, Humphrey L, Muhammad RF, Zhao JX, Peng LC (2013) First discovery of Pleistocene orangutan (Pongo sp.) fossils in Peninsular Malaysia: biogeographic and paleoenvironmental implications. J Hum Evol 65:770–797

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. McCann C, Buchanan-Smith HM, Jones-Engel L, Farmer KH, Prescott MJ, Fitch-Snyder H, Taylor S (2007) IPS International guidelines for the acquisition, care and breeding of nonhuman primates, 2nd edn. International Primatological Society, USA, pp 1–76

    Google Scholar 

  19. Kanamori T, Kuze N, Bernard H, Malim TP, Kohshima S (2010) Feeding ecology of Bornean orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus morio) in Danum Valley, Sabah, Malaysia: a 3-year record including two mast fruitings. Am J Primatol 72:820–840

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Kanamori T, Kuze N, Bernard H, Malim TP, Kohshima S (2017) Fluctuations of population density in Bornean orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus morio) related to fruit availability in the Danum Valley, Sabah, Malaysia: a 10-year record including two mast fruitings and three other peak fruitings. Primates 58:225–235

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Kuze N, Sipangkui S, Malim TP, Bernard H, Ambu LN, Kohshima S (2008) Reproductive parameters over a 37-year period of free-ranging female Borneo orangutans at Sepilok Orangutan Rehabilitation Centre. Primates 49:126–134

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Kuze N, Dellatore D, Banes GL, Pratje P, Tajima T, Russon AE (2012) Factors affecting reproduction in rehabilitant female orangutans: young age at first birth and short inter-birth interval. Primates 53:181–192

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Labes EM, Hegglin D, Grimm F, Nurcahyo W, Harrison ME, Bastian ML, Deplazes P (2010) Intestinal parasites of endangered orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) in Central and East Kalimantan, Borneo, Indonesia. Parasitology 137:123–135

    CAS  Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Leighton M (1993) Modeling dietary selectivity by Bornean orangutans: evidence for integration of multiple criteria in fruit selection. Int J Primatol 14:257–313

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Martin P, Bateson P (2007) Measuring behaviour: an introductory guide, 3rd edn. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Book  Google Scholar 

  26. Mashhor M, Anuar MSS (2013) The involvement of Universiti Sains Malaysia in research of orang-utan (Pongo pygmaeus) at Tasik Bukit Merah, Taiping, Perak. In: Latiff A, Mashhor M (eds) Ex situ conservation of orang utan. Bukit Merah Orang Utan Island Foundation, Bukit Merah, pp 121–127

    Google Scholar 

  27. Mendonça RS, Kanamori T, Kuze N, Hayashi M, Bernard H, Matsuzawa T (2017) Development and behavior of wild infant-juvenile East Bornean orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus morio) in Danum Valley. Primates 58:211–224

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Morrogh-Bernard HC, Husson SJ, Knott CD, Wich SA, van Schaik CP, van Noordwijk MA, Lackman-Ancrenaz I, Marshall AJ, Kanamori T, Kuze N, bin Sakong R (2009) Orangutan activity budgets and diet. In: Wich SA, Atmoko SU, Setia TM, van Schaik CP (eds) Orangutans: geographic variation in behavioral ecology and conservation. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 119–133

    Google Scholar 

  29. Robins JG, Ancrenaz M, Parker J, Goossens B, Ambu L, Walzer C (2013) The release of northeast Bornean orangutans to Tabin Wildlife Reserve, Sabah, Malaysia. In: Soorae PS (ed) Global re-introduction perspectives: 2013. Further case studies from around the globe. IUCN/SSC Re-introduction Specialist Group and Environment Agency-Abu Dhabi, Gland, pp 215–221

    Google Scholar 

  30. Russon AE (2002) Return of the native: cognition and site-specific expertise in orangutan rehabilitation. Int J Primatol 23:461–478

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Russon A (2009a) Orangutans. Curr Biol 19:R925–R927

    CAS  Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Russon AE (2009b) Orangutan rehabilitation and reintroduction. In: Wich SA, Atmoko SU, Setia TM, van Schaik CP (eds) Orangutans: geographic variation in behavioral ecology and conservation. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 327–350

    Google Scholar 

  33. Russon AE, Wich SA, Ancrenaz M, Kanamori T, Knott CD, Kuze N, Morrogh-Bernard HC, Pratje P, Ramlee H, Rodman P, Sawang A, Sidiyasa K, Singleton I, van Schaik CP (2007) Orangutan foods 2007. http://www.yorku.ca/arusson/. Accessed 11 Jan 2017

  34. Russon AE, Wich SA, Ancrenaz M, Kanamori T, Knott CD, Kuze N, Morrogh-Bernard HC, Pratje P, Ramlee H, Rodman P, Sawang A, Sidiyasa K, Singleton I, van Schaik CP (2009) Geographic variation in orangutan diets. In: Wich SA, Atmoko SU, Setia TM, van Schaik CP (eds) Orangutans: geographic variation in behavioral ecology and conservation. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 135–156

    Google Scholar 

  35. Sabapathy D (2013) Veterinary management of infant orang utan at the Orang Utan Island, Bukit Merah Laketown Resort. In: Latiff A, Mashhor M (eds) Ex situ conservation of orang utan. Bukit Merah Orang Utan Island Foundation, Bukit Merah, pp 128–133

    Google Scholar 

  36. Smith JJ (2009) Orangutan–human interaction in rehabilitation: orangutan contribution to interaction and conflict. Master thesis in York University

  37. Tshen LT (2016) Biogeographic distribution and metric dental variation of fossil and living orangutans (Pongo spp.). Primates 57:39–50

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. van Adrichem GGJ, Utami SS, Wich SA, van Hooff JARAM, Sterck EHM (2006) The development of wild immature Sumatran orangutans (Pongo abelii) at Ketambe. Primates 47:300–309

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. van Noordwijk M, van Schaik CP (2005) Development of ecological competence in Sumatran orangutans. Am J Physic Anthropol 127:79–94

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. van Schaik CP, Marshall AJ, Wich SA (2009) Geographic variation in orangutan behavior and biology. In: Wich SA, Atmoko SU, Setia TM, van Schaik CP (eds) Orangutans: geographic variation in behavioral ecology and conservation. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 351–361

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The present study was supported by a Grant-Aid for Scientific Research from JSPS #20002001, #24000001, and #16H06283 to T. Matsuzawa, #23220006 and #15H05709 to M. Tomonaga, #19700245, #23700313 and #15K00204 to MH, Core-to-Core Program A. Advanced Research Networks (CCSN to T. Matsuzawa). Thanks are due to Tetsuro Matsuzawa, Masaki Tomonaga, Hideko Takeshita, Shiro Kohshima, Noko Kuze, Tomoko Kanamori, Hidetoshi Kurotori, Kiyonori Kumazaki, Mari Hirosawa, Yumi Yamanashi, Yoko Sakuraba, Etsuko Ichino, Renata Mendonça, Yui Fujimori, Tomoko Takashima, Emi Yamamoto, Aya Saito, and the staff members at the Section of Language and Intelligence and Center for Human Evolution Modeling Research in Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University for their support and helpful comments on the present study. Special thanks to the Malaysian collaborators: Tan Sri Mustapha Kamal Abu Bakar, Daniel Baskaran, Mohamed Nasir Sulaiman, Shafie Abd Aziz, Siti Norhayati Yahaya, Nurul Fadhilah Bt Soad, Nor Fadzila Ahmad Puzi, Divanee A/P Raman, Muhamad Zawawi Abd Rahman, Abdul Aziz Shamsudin, Noorkhairiah Salleh, and other staff working for OUI. Tree census was conducted with the initiative role taken by Najimi Hassan. The care and management of OUI orangutans followed the advice from the Board of Trustees of Bukit Merah Orang Utan Island Foundation and complied with Malaysian Law.

Author information

Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Misato Hayashi.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (DOCX 36 kb)

Rights and permissions

This article is published under an open access license. Please check the 'Copyright Information' section either on this page or in the PDF for details of this license and what re-use is permitted. If your intended use exceeds what is permitted by the license or if you are unable to locate the licence and re-use information, please contact the Rights and Permissions team.

About this article

Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Hayashi, M., Kawakami, F., Roslan, R. et al. Behavioral studies and veterinary management of orangutans at Bukit Merah Orang Utan Island, Perak, Malaysia. Primates 59, 135–144 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10329-018-0650-2

Download citation

Keywords

  • Behavioral monitoring
  • Ex situ conservation
  • Orangutan
  • Veterinary management