Skip to main content
Log in

Negative expertise: comparing differently tenured elder care nurses' negative knowledge

  • Published:
European Journal of Psychology of Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

An Erratum to this article was published on 10 March 2011

Abstract

Negative expertise is conceptualised as the professional's ability to avoid errors during practice due to certain cognitive agencies. In this study, negative knowledge (i.e. knowledge about what is wrong in a certain context and situation) is conceptualised as one such agency. This study compares and investigates the negative knowledge of elder care nurses at three different levels of professional experience. Thereby, various forms of negative knowledge—declarative, procedural, self-reflective and vicarious—are investigated. Moreover, the specificity of negative knowledge is compared across different levels of professional experience. Verbal data was collected from a prompting task study of 37 elder care nurses. These were prompted 20 diagnoses of varying typicality for the professional field. The nurses were asked what was critical to pay attention to and what ought to be avoided in case of these diagnoses. The study's results reveal a significant superiority of highly tenured elder care nurses in vicarious negative knowledge, as well as in highly specified negative knowledge. Procedural and procedural negative knowledge (PNK)—which are the most important facets in quantitative terms—show an approximately parallel developmental pattern among the different groups. For declarative knowledge, a U-shaped intermediate effect was discovered. It is concluded that PNK has the most immediate error-preventive function. With regards to future research, the great specificity of highly tenured elder care nurses' negative knowledge discovered in this study is discussed here as one possible explanation as to why they are able to intentionally avoid errors (i.e. their negative expertise).

Résumé

L'expertise négative est conceptualisée en tant que la capacité professionnelle à éviter les erreurs lors de la pratique dues à certains agents cognitifs. Dans cette étude, les connaissances négatives (c.-à-d. les connaissances relatives à ce qui est erroné dans un contexte ou une situation donné) sont conceptualisées en tant qu'agent de ce type. Cette étude compare et étudie les connaissances négatives des infirmières prodiguant des soins aux personnes âgées à trois stades d'expérience professionnelle différents. Nous étudions donc plusieurs formes de connaissances négatives, à savoir les connaissances déclaratives, procédurales, auto-réflectives et vicariantes. De plus, la spécificité des connaissances négatives est comparée en fonction des différents niveaux d'expérience. Des données verbales ont été collectées à partir d'une étude de tâches proposées à 37 infirmières soignant des personnes âgées. On leur a proposé 20 diagnostics de typicalité variable dans leur domaine professionnel. Il a été demandé aux infirmières ce qu'il était crucial de surveiller avec attention et ce qui devait être évité dans le cas de ces diagnostics. Les résultats de cette étude révèlent une supériorité importante des infirmières soignant des personnes âgées les plus gradées en matière de connaissances négatives vicariantes ainsi qu'en ce qui concerne les connaissances négatives très spécifiques. Les connaissances procédurales et les connaissances négatives procédurales, qui sont les facettes les plus importantes en termes de quantité, montrent un schéma de développement approximativement parallèle entre les différents groupes. Pour les connaissances déclaratives, un effet intermédiaire en forme de U a été constaté. On en arrive à la conclusion que les connaissances négatives procédurales possèdent la fonction de prévention des erreurs la plus immédiate. En ce qui concerne la recherche future, la grande spécificité des connaissances négatives des infirmières soignant des personnes âgées les plus gradées révélées dans cette étude est exposée ici comme étant une explication possible de la raison pour laquelle elles sont capables d'éviter intentionnellement des erreurs (c.-à-d. leur expertise négative).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Anderson, J. R. (1982). Acquisition of cognitive skill. Psychological Review, 89, 369–406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J. R., & Lebiere, C. (1998). The atomic components of thought. Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bauer, J. (2008). Learning from errors at work. Studies on nurses' engagement in error related learning activities. Doctoral dissertation, University of Regensburg, Regensburg. Retrieved June 8, 2009, from http://www.opus-bayern.de/uni-regensburg/volltexte/2008/990.

  • Bengtsson, J. (1995). What is reflection? On reflection in the teaching profession and teacher education. Teachers and Teaching: Theory and Practice, 1, 23–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benner, P. (1982). From novice to expert. The American Journal of Nursing, 82, 402–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benner, P. (1994). The role of articulation in understanding practice and experience as sources of knowledge in clinical nursing. In C. Taylor, J. Tully, & D. Weinstock (Eds.), Philosophy in an age of pluralism: The philosophy of Charles Taylor revisited (pp. 136–155). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berragan, L. (1998). Nursing practice draws upon several different ways of knowing. Journal of Clinical Nursing, 7, 209–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Billett, S. (1999). Experts' ways of knowing. Australian Vocational Education Review, 6, 25–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bonner, A. (2007). Understanding the role of knowledge in the practice of expert nephrology nurses in Australia. Nursing & Health Sciences, 9, 161–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boshuizen, H. P. A., & Schmidt, H. G. (1992). On the role of biomedical knowledge in clinical reasoning by experts, intermediates and novices. Cognitive Science, 16, 153–184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brewer, W. F. (1986). What is autobiographical memory? In D. Rubin (Ed.), Autobiographical memory (pp. 25–49). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bromme, R., Rambow, R., & Sträßer, R. (1996). Jenseits von „Oberfläche“ und „Tiefe“: Zum Zusammenhang von Problemkategorisierungen und Arbeitskontext bei Fachleuten des Technischen Zeichnens [Beyond “surface” and “depth”: The relationship between problem categorisations and work context at technical drawing experts]. In H. Gruber & A. Ziegler (Eds.), Expertiseforschung: Theoretische und methodische Grundlagen (pp. 150–168). Opladen: Westdeutscher.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burnard, P. (1987). Towards an epistemological basis for experiential learning in nurse education. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 12, 189–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carpenito, L. (1983). Nursing diagnosis. Applications to clinical practice. Philadelphia: Lippincott.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carper, B. A. (1978). Fundamental patterns of knowing in nursing. Advances in Nursing Science, 1, 13–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cianciolo, A. T., Matthew, C., Sternberg, R. J., & Wagner, R. K. (2006). Tacit knowledge, practical intelligence, and expertise. In K. A. Ericsson, N. Charness, P. J. Feltovich, & R. R. Hoffman (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance (pp. 613–632). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conway, J. E. (1998). Evolution of the species “expert nurse”. An examination of the practical knowledge held by expert nurses. Journal of Clinical Nursing, 7, 75–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corcoran, S. A. (1986). Task complexity and nursing expertise as factors of decision making. Nursing Research, 35, 107–112.

    Google Scholar 

  • Custers, E. J. F. M., Boshuizen, H. P. A., & Schmidt, H. G. (1999). The role of illness scripts in the development of medical expertise: Results from an interview study. Cognition and Instruction, 16, 367–398.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Doenges, M., Moorhouse, M. F., & Geissler-Murr, A. (2002). Pflegediagnosen und Maßnahmen [Nursing diagnoses and measures]. Bern: Huber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehrenfeld, M., Tabak, N., Bronner, G., & Bergman, R. (1997). Ethical dilemmas concerning the sexuality of elderly patients suffering from dementia. International Journal of Nursing Practice, 3, 255–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Endsley, M. R. (2006). Expertise and situation awareness. In K. A. Ericsson, N. Charness, P. J. Feltovich, & R. R. Hoffman (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance (pp. 633–651). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K. A. (2003). The search for general abilities and basic capacities: Theoretical implications from the modifiability and complexity of mechanisms mediating expert performance. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Psychology of abilities, competencies, and expertise (pp. 93–125). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K. A., Charness, P. J., Feltovich, P. J., & Hoffmann, R. R. (Eds.). (2006). The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K. A., & Lehmann, A. C. (1996). Expert and exceptional performance: Evidence of maximal adaptation to task constraints. Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 273–305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K. A., & Simon, H. (1993). Protocol analysis: Verbal reports as data. Cambridge: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K. A., Whyte, J., & Ward, P. (2007). Expert performance in nursing: Reviewing research on expertise in nursing within the framework of the expert performance approach. Advances in Nursing Science, 30, E58–E71.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frese, M., & Zapf, D. (1994). Action as the core of work psychology: A German approach. In H. C. Triandis, M. D. Dunette, & L. M. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology (2nd ed., Vol. 4, pp. 271–340). Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gartmeier, M., Bauer, J., Gruber, H., & Heid, H. (2008). Negative knowledge: Understanding professional learning and expertise. Vocations and Learning, 1, 87–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gartmeier, M., Gruber, H. & Heid, H. (2010). Tracing error-related knowledge in interview data: Negative knowledge in elder care nursing. Educational Gerontology, 36(9).

  • Gustafsson, C., & Fagerberg, I. (2004). Reflection, the way to professional development? Journal of Clinical Nursing, 13, 271–280.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamers, J. P. H., van den Hout, M. A., Halfens, R. J. G., Abbus-Saad, H. H., & Heijltjes, A. E. G. (1997). Differences in pain assessment and decisions regarding the administration of analgesics between novices, intermediates and experts in pediatric nursing. International Journal of Nursing Studies, 34, 325–334.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hodges, N. J., Starkes, J. L., & MacMahon, C. (2006). Expert performance in sport: A cognitive perspective. In K. A. Ericsson, N. Charness, P. J. Feltovich, & R. R. Hoffman (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance (pp. 471–488). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, E. (2006). Expertise, talent, and social encouragement. In K. A. Ericsson, N. Charness, P. J. Feltovich, & R. R. Hoffman (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance (pp. 31–38). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jasper, M. A. (1994). Expert: A discussion of the implications of the concept as used in nursing. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 20, 769–776.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, P. E., Duran, A. S., Hassebrock, F., Moller, J. H., Pritula, M., Feltovich, P. J., et al. (1981). Expertise and error in diagnostic reasoning. Cognitive Science, 5, 235–283.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kasch, C. R. (1986). Toward a theory of nursing action: Skills and competency in nurse–patient interaction. Nursing Research, 35, 226–230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kolodner, J. (1983). Towards an understanding of the role of experience in the evolution from novice to expert. International Journal of Man-Machine Studies, 19, 497–518.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lobchuk, M. (2005). Concept analysis of perspective taking: Meeting informal caregiver needs for communication competence and accurate perception. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 54, 330–341.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maggs, C., & Biley, A. (2000). Reflections on the role of the nursing development facilitator in clinical supervision and reflective practice. International Journal of Nursing Practice, 6, 192–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McConnell, E. (2002). Applying antiembolism stockings. Nursing, 32, 17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meurier, C. E., Vincent, C. A., & Parmar, D. G. (1997). Learning from errors in nursing practice. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 26, 111–119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Minsky, M. (1997). Negative expertise. In P. J. Feltovich, K. M. Ford, & R. R. Hoffman (Eds.), Expertise in context: Human and machine (pp. 515–521). Cambridge: AAAI/MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moch, S. D. (1990). Personal knowing: Evolving research and practice. Scholarly Inquiry for Nursing Practice, 4, 155–165.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murtonen, M., Sahlström, S., Tynjälä, P., et al. (2009). Educating novices at the workplace: Transformation of conceptions and skills of students on a metal industry course. In M. L. Stenström & P. Tynjälä (Eds.), Towards integration of work and learning. Strategies for connectivity and transformation (pp. 93–113). Dordrecht: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • National Transportation Safety Board (NTSB). (1998). 1997 U.S. Airline fatalities down substantially from previous year; general aviation deaths rise. Retrieved June 8, 2009, from http://www.ntsb.gov/Pressrel/1998/980224_2.htm.

  • Neno, R., Aveyard, B., Heath, H., & Baldwin, H. (2007). Introduction. In R. Neno, B. Aveyard, & H. Heath (Eds.), Older people and mental health nursing (pp. XV–XVI). Oxford: Blackwell.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Nievelstein, F., Van Gog, T., Boshuizen, H. P. A., & Prins, F. J. (2008). Expertise-related differences in conceptual and ontological knowledge in the legal domain. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 20, 1043–1064.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oser, F., & Spychiger, M. (2005). Lernen ist schmerzhaft. Zur Theorie des negativen Wissens und zur Praxis der Fehlerkultur [Learning is painful. On the theory of negative knowledge and the practice of error culture]. Weinheim: Beltz.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parviainen, J., & Eriksson, M. (2006). Negative knowledge, expertise and organisations. International Journal of Management Concepts and Philosophy, 2, 140–153.

    Google Scholar 

  • Patel, V. L., & Ramoni, M. F. (1997). Cognitive models of directional inference in expert medical reasoning. In P. J. Feltovich, K. M. Ford, & R. R. Hoffman (Eds.), Expertise in context: Human and machine (pp. 68–99). Cambridge: AAAI/MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pintrich, P. R. (2002). The role of metacognitive knowledge in learning, teaching and assessing. Theory into Practice, 41, 219–225.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prümper, J. (1991). Handlungsfehler und Expertise [Mistakes and expertise]. In M. Frese & D. Zapf (Eds.), Fehler bei der Arbeit mit dem Computer (pp. 118–130). Bern: Huber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reason, J. T. (1990). Human error. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reason, J. T. (1995). Understanding adverse events: Human factors. Quality in Health Care, 4, 80–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rikers, R., Schmidt, H. G., & Boshuizen, H. P. A. (2000). Knowledge encapsulation and the intermediate effect. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25, 150–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sandelowski, M. (2003). Taking things seriously: Studying the material culture of nursing. In J. Latimer (Ed.), Advanced qualitative research for nursing (pp. 185–210). Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schank, R. C. (1999). Dynamic memory revisited. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, H. G., & Boshuizen, H. P. A. (1993). On the origin of intermediate effects in clinical case recall. Memory & Cognition, 21, 338–351.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sonnentag, S., Niessen, C., & Volmer, J. (2006). Expertise in software design. In K. A. Ericsson, N. Charness, P. J. Feltovich, & R. R. Hoffman (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance (pp. 373–387). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stark, R., Mandl, H., Gruber, H., & Renkl, A. (1998). Indeed, sometimes knowledge does not help: A replication study. Instructional Science, 26, 391–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stokes, A. F., Kemper, K., & Kite, K. (1997). Aeronautical decision making, cue recognition, and expertise under time pressure. In C. E. Zsambok & G. Klein (Eds.), Naturalistic decision making (pp. 183–196). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strack, F., & Förster, J. (1998). Self-reflection and recognition: The role of metacognitive knowledge in the attribution of recollective experience. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2, 111–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strasser, J. (2006). Erfahrung und Wissen in der Beratung [Experience and knowledge in counselling]. Göttingen: Cuvillier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tanner, C. A., Padrick, K. P., Westfall, U. E., & Putzier, D. J. (1987). Diagnostic reasoning studies of nurses and nursing students. Nursing Research, 36, 358–363.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van de Wiel, M. (1997). Knowledge encapsulation. Studies on the development of medical expertise. Wageningen: Ponsen & Looijen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wentz, J. D., Karch, A. M., & Karch, F. E. (2000). You've caught the error—Now how do you fix it? The do's and don'ts of changing an order. The American Journal of Nursing, 100, 24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whyte, J., Ward, P., & Eccles, D. W. (2009). The relationship between knowledge and clinical performance in novice and experienced critical care nurses. Heart & Lung, 38, 517–525.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (German Research Foundation), grant Gr 1384/11-2, awarded to Hans Gruber and Helmut Heid.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Martin Gartmeier.

Additional information

Martin Gartmeier. TUM School of Education, Susanne Klatten-Stiftungslehrstuhl für Empirische Bildungsforschung, c/o Martin Gartmeier TU München, Schellingstraße 33, 80799 München. E-mail: martin.gartmeier@tum.de; Web site: http://www.ebf.edu.tum.de/

Current themes of research:

Workplace learning. Learning from errors and negative knowledge. Professional communication.

Most relevant publications in the field of Psychology of Education:

Gartmeier, M., Bauer, J., Gruber, H., & Heid, H. (2010). Workplace errors and negative knowledge in elder care nursing. Human Resource Development International, 13, 5–25.

Gartmeier, M., Kipfmüller, S., Gruber, H., & Heid, H. (2009). Fehlerorientierung und Eigeninitiative im Bankensektor [Error orientation and personal initiative in the banking sector]. Zeitschrift für Arbeits- und Organisationspsychologie, 53, 149–162.

Gartmeier, M., Bauer, J., Gruber, H., & Heid, H. (2008). Negative knowledge: Understanding professional learning and expertise. Vocations and Learning, 2, 87–103.

Erno Lehtinen. University of Turku, Center for Learning Research, 20014 Turun yliopisto, Finland. E-mail: erno.lehtinen@utu.de; Web site: http://users.utu.fi/ernoleh/

Current themes of research:

Research on learning environments and professional networks. Educational technology. Expertise development.

Most relevant publications in the field of Psychology of Education:

Murtonen, M., Olkinuora, E., Palonen, T., Hakkarainen, K., and Lehtinen, E. (2008). Motivational orientation to work in different types of organizations. International Journal of Educational Research, 47(4), 213–222.

Lehtinen, E. (2008). Bridging the individual and social in workplace learning and motivation. International Journal of Educational Research, 47(4), 261–263.

Gruber, H., Lehtinen, E., Palonen, T., & Degner, S. (2008). Persons in the shadow: Assessing the social context of high abilities. Psychology Science Quarterly, 50(2), 237–258.

Hannula, M.M., & Lehtinen, E. (2005). Spontaneous focusing on numerosity and mathematical skills of young children. Learning and Instruction, 15(3), 237–256

Hans Gruber. University of Regensburg, Institute of Educational Research, Unit Prof. Dr. H. Gruber, 93040 Regensburg, Germany. E-mail: Hans.Gruber(@)paedagogik.uni-regensburg.de; Web site: http://www-campus.uni-r.de/edu3/

Current themes of research:

Professional learning. Expertise. Negative knowledge. Network analysis. Higher education.

Most relevant publications in the field of Psychology of Education:

Gegenfurtner, A., Veermans, K., Festner, D., & Gruber, H. (2009). Motivation to transfer training: An integrative literature review. Human Resource Development Review, 8, 403–423.

Gruber, H., Jansen, P., Marienhagen, J., & Altenmüller, E. (2010). Adaptations during the acquisition of expertise. Talent Development and Excellence, 2, 3–15.

Harteis, C., Bauer, J., & Gruber, H. (2008). The culture of learning from mistakes: How employees handle mistakes in everyday work. International Journal of Educational Research, 47, 223–231.

Rupprecht, M., Strasser, J., Gruber, H., & Harteis, C. (2010). Expertise of team leaders in analysing team conflicts. Vocations and Learning: Studies in Vocational and Professional Learning, 3, 39–54.

Helmut Heid. University of Regensburg, Institute for Educational Research, c/o Prof. em. Dr. Helmut Heid, 93040 Regensburg, Germany. E-mail: Helmut.Heid@paedagogik.uni-regensburg.de; Web site: http://www-campus.uni-r.de/edu3/

Current themes of research:

Educational equity. Mutual relationships between the educational and employment system. Philosophy of educational science. Error culture.

Most relevant publications in the field of Psychology of Education:

Harteis, C., Bauer, J., & Heid, H. (2006). Der Umgang mit Fehlern als Merkmal betrieblicher Fehlerkultur und Voraussetzung für Professional Learning. Schweizerische Zeitschrift für Bildungswissenschaften, 28, 111–129.

Heid, H., Gruber, H. (2005). Personnel and organisational development from an economic and an educational perspective. In: Gruber, H. (Ed.), Bridging individual, organisational, and cultural aspects of professional learning. Regensburg: Roderer.

Heid, H. (2005). Übertragung von Verantwortung—die Verwandlung fremdbestimmten Sollens in selbstbestimmtes Wollen. In: M. Eigenstetter & M. Hammerl (Eds.), Wirtschafts- und Unternehmensethik—ein Widerspruch in sich? (pp. 163–182). Kröning: Asanger.

Heid, H. (2004). The domestication of critique: Problems of justifying the critical in the context of educationally relevant thought and action. Journal of Philosophy of Education, 38(3), 323–339.

Bauer, J., Festner, D., Gruber, H., Harteis, C. & Heid, H. (2004). The effects of epistemological beliefs on workplace learning. Journal of Workplace Learning, 16, 284–292.

An erratum to this article can be found at http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10212-011-0057-6

Appendix

Appendix

Categorical scheme developed for analysing the verbal data (with categories of negative knowledge printed in italics):

  • Declarative knowledge:

    • Enabling conditions, causes of certain diagnoses

    • Prevalence (frequency of occurrence)

    • Symptoms, appearance and course of a diagnosis

    • Classification and differential diagnosis

    • DNK (common misconceptions with regards to certain diagnosis; non-similarity of diagnoses)

  • Procedural knowledge:

    • General, strategic, goal-related

    • Concrete measures and actions

    • Contextual procedural knowledge (including other professions, modifying actions due to special circumstances)

    • PNK (to know what not to do; to know about what must not be omitted; to know which situations should be avoided)

  • Self-reflective knowledge:

    • Personal role, own competence

    • Elaborating own experiences and opinions

    • SRNK (to know about limitations of one's own influence; to know about limitations of one's own knowledge or skills)

  • Vicarious and episodic knowledge:

    • Individuality, biography or special needs of certain (groups of) residents

    • Generalised episode, example

    • Distinct episode

    • VNK (to know what somebody is not able to; to know what somebody does not recognise or understand)

  • Specificity of negative knowledge:

    • Global reference

    • Reference to a special diagnosis

    • More specific reference

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Gartmeier, M., Lehtinen, E., Gruber, H. et al. Negative expertise: comparing differently tenured elder care nurses' negative knowledge. Eur J Psychol Educ 26, 273–300 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10212-010-0042-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10212-010-0042-5

Keywords

Navigation