First, I found that jays did not respond to the blackbird songs, but to mobbing calls, which confirms hypothesis 1 and suggests that jays eavesdrop on the signal of a blackbird directed towards a predator, but not towards the presence of the blackbird itself when the predation context is lacking (similar to Forsman and Mönkkönen 2001 for other species). Thus, this study adds to previous studies demonstrating heterospecific eavesdropping in a mobbing context (Goodale et al. 2010; Magrath et al. 2020). The data of mobbing playbacks was pooled across different predators to generalize across different predator threats (comparable to Randler 2007). Further studies now should assess responses of jays towards different predators of blackbirds. Some of these predators may preferably predate on nest and young (e.g., magpie), others on adults, and some on both (cats). However, the detailed response towards different blackbird playbacks was not in the focus of the study.
The study further suggests that jays have incomplete information about the reason of the blackbird mobbing in the playback-only experiment, because they approached closer and stayed longer in the playback-only experiment as the exact position or identity of the predator is unknown. When the respective reason of the mobbing activity is presented by including a model predator, jays approached less close and stayed shorter, because the information about the predator and its location is complete, confirming hypothesis 2. Thus, the study shows that jays need extra information to make an informed decision about their response towards the mobbing calls of blackbirds. Comparably, in yellowhammers Emberiza citrinella, individuals with less complete information about a predator exhibited alert perching more often immediately after the encounter than did birds that saw a predator model (van der Veen 2002). Generally, obtaining information by directly observing a situation is more reliable than indirect information obtained from the behavioral decisions of other individuals (Giraldeau et al. 2002; Barrera et al. 2011; Magrath et al. 2009).
Many animals use heterospecific calls to gain information about the current predation risk, but not always in a reciprocal manner. In some cases, this is considered as information exploitation. Such exploitation has been documented, e.g., in Downy Woodpeckers, Picoides pubescens (Sullivan 1984), where woodpeckers reduced their vigilance when playbacks of heterospecific contact calls have been broadcast. The heterospecific contact calls were perceived as a cue of safety. Furthermore, downy woodpeckers did not respond to predator presentations with alarm calling (only in cases when a conspecific of the opposite sex was present), while tufted titmice, Baeolophus bicolor, and black-capped chickadees, Poecile atricapillus, often gave alarm calls in predator contexts. Thus, the downy woodpeckers exploited the information provided by the smaller songbirds but did not “pay back” by own alarm calling (Sullivan 1985). Playback experiments with Western Australian magpies (Cractucus tibicen dorsalis), for example, showed that magpies take into account the reliability of a caller (Silvestri et al. 2019). Magpies were exposed to alarm calls of two group members: one was played in the presence of a predator model while the other was not. In a second trial, playbacks of the group members were presented without a predator model and the receiver responded stronger to signallers that previously gave alarm calls in the presence of a predator model. Nuthatches, Sitta europaea, alter the acoustic features according to whether they gained direct or indirect information about a threat. When receiving direct information, they produced mobbing calls reflecting the given threat. However, when obtaining indirect information, they only produce calls with intermediate acoustic features, suggesting that this species is sensitive to the source and reliability of information (Carlson et al. 2020). The behavior of jays when hearing blackbird mobbing calls can also be explained by information exploitation, but the data are only suggestive about that and need further scrutinization. To test for exploitation, it is important to see if the receivers help the signaler (i.e., by mobbing) or not (exploitation). This needs to be tested explicitly with analyses of mobbing behavior including wing flicking, tail flicking, and other mobbing behaviors. In this study, only obvious mobbing behavior was studied (attack flights, diving towards the model), but such behavior did not occur, and future studies should focus on these more subtle behaviors. Also, when presenting only blackbird mobbing calls, jays—even when they wanted to participate in mobbing—have no target for an attack because attacking the speaker would mean attacking the blackbird, not the predator. An interesting additional experiment would be to present jays with the model predators only.
Alternatively, jays might also use mobbing calls of blackbirds during the breeding season to eavesdrop to locate a blackbird’s nest to prey upon the eggs or young. Future studies might investigate this possibility by testing if jays search for a blackbird’s nest after hearing mobbing calls if no model predator is present. Then, jays should not mob, but rather approach carefully to search for the nest. In this case, however, jays should stay longer within the area to try to locate the nest, and artificial nests of blackbirds should be preyed upon by jays more often when blackbird mobbing calls are played back. In turn, the reaction of the blackbirds would be interesting, because the blackbirds may be aggressive to the jays if they did approach even if they would help mobbing. In this case, one would expect seasonal differences in mobbing, with fewer approaches of jays during the breeding season and more during winter.