Abstract
This paper examines the relationship between individuals’ experience of corruption and their anxiety using microeconomic data from the Afrobarometer surveys. The results show a statistically significant and economically meaningful relationship in probit models using both an experience of corruption index and a simple dummy variable. Having to pay a bribe to obtain documents and permits, to avoid problems with the police or to access medical care are the scenarios in which this relationship is strongest. Some evidence is presented that an individual needs to experience such corruption more than ‘once or twice’ for these relationships to become evident.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.



Notes
The full data and methodology as well as summary statistics can be obtained from www.afrobarometer.org.
The Round 2 survey for Mozambique allowed the additional response of ‘always.’ As only a tiny proportion of the sample opted for this option in any of the corruption questions and it was not an option in other countries, I add those who did to the ‘often’ category.
Round 2 asks about paying a bribe to cross a border. I opt not to use this as it is not possible to tell if the bribe is paid to agents of the respondents own country or of another and it was not asked in Mozambique.
Constructing the index using a principal components approach does not alter the results.
While Round 3 does ask about school expenses, I omit this as there are many things which could fall under this category that we may not wish to include in a poverty index such as private school fees.
While it is far from clear that people have a common understanding of ‘always’ and ‘many times’, I am potentially ignoring information by using a dummy variable to measure anxiety. The results of an ordered probit model support the results presented here and are available on request.
Using the Round 2 data, Graham and Hoover (2007) find a negative effect of crime on living conditions. The main difference in their specification is that they use the data on income decile as opposed to a lived poverty index. When I do likewise, I too find that crime and corruption are detrimental to self-reported living conditions. However, when I include both lived poverty and income, neither crime nor corruption is significant at the 5 % level, though corruption is significant at 10 %.
32 % of the respondents choose this response for the documents and permits question, 28 % for the school placement question, 36 % for the household services question, 22 % for the accessing medicine or medical treatment question and 32 % for the avoiding problems with the police question.
Another way to tackle this issue is to use dummies which contrast those with no experience with those who do but did not experience corruption. The results from this exercise lead to the same conclusions as those presented here.
The physical health dummy is created from the question ‘in the last month, how much of the time has your physical health reduced the amount of work you normally do inside or outside your home?’ The dummy takes a value of one if the respondent answers ‘many times’ or ‘always’ and zero otherwise.
References
Alesina A, Di Tella R, MacCulloch R (2004) Inequality and happiness: are Europeans and Americans different? J Public Econ 88(9):2009–2042
Ari A (2008) Some educational sources of anxiety in Kyrgyz youth. Soc Behav Personal Int J 36(5):577–584
Azfar O, Gurgur T (2008) Does corruption affect health outcomes in the Philippines? Econ Gov 9(3):197–244
Banerjee A, Duflo E (2011) Poor economics: a radical rethinking of the way to fight global poverty (PublicAffairs)
Ebrahim A, Botha F, Snowball J (2013) Determinants of life satisfaction among race groups in South Africa. Dev South Afr 30(2):168–185
Gardner J, Oswald AJ (2007) Money and mental wellbeing: a longitudinal study of medium-sized lottery wins. J Health Econ 26(1):49–60
Gambetti E, Giusberti F (2012) The effect of anger and anxiety traits on investment decisions. J Econ Psychol 33(6):1059–1069
Graham C (2009) Happiness around the world: the paradox of happy peasants and miserable millionaires. Oxford University Press, Oxford
Graham C, Hoover M (2007) Optimism and poverty in Africa: adaptation or a means to survival? Working paper 76, Afrobarometer
Graham C, Chattopadhyay S (2009) Well-being and public attitudes in Afghanistan: some insights from the economics of happiness. Working papers 2, Foreign Policy at Brookings
Greenberg PE, Sisitsky T, Kessler RC, Finkelstein SN, Berndt ER, Davidson JR, Ballenger JC, Fyer AJ (1999) The economic burden of anxiety disorders in the 1990s. J Clin Psychiatry 60(7):427–435
Heflin CM, Siefert K, Williams DR (2005) Food insufficiency and women’s mental health: findings from a 3-year panel of welfare recipients. Soc Sci Med 61(9):1971–1982
Helliwell JF, Putnam RD (2004) The social context of well-being. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 359(1449):1435
Marciniak M, Lage MJ, Landbloom RP, Dunayevich E, Bowman L (2004) Medical and productivity costs of anxiety disorders: case control study. Depress Anxiety 19:112–120
Mattes R, Bratton M, Davids YD (2003) Poverty, survival, and democracy in Southern Africa. Working paper 23, Afrobarometer
Møller V, Jackson A (1997) Perceptions of service delivery and happiness. Dev South Afr 14(2):169–184
Pedigo KL, Marshall V (2009) Bribery: Australian managers’ experiences and responses when operating in international markets. J Bus Ethics 87(1):59–74
Peng J, Xiao W, Yang Y, Wu S, Miao D (2014) The impact of trait anxiety on self-frame and decision making. J Behav Decis Mak 27(1):11–19
Porcelli AJ, Delgado MR (2009) Acute stress modulates risk taking in financial decision making. Psychol Sci 20(3):278
Sen A (1999) Development as freedom. Oxford University Press, Oxford
Shavell S (1993) An economic analysis of threats and their illegality: blackmail, extortion, and robbery. University of Pennsylvania Law Review, pp 1877–1903
Stafford M, Chandola T, Marmot M (2007) Association between fear of crime and mental health and physical functioning. Am J Public Health 97(11):2076
Tavits M (2008) Representation, corruption, and subjective well-being. Comp Polit Stud 41(12):1607
van den Bos R, Harteveld M, Stoop H (2009) Stress and decision-making in humans: performance is related to cortisol reactivity, albeit differently in men and women. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34(10):1449–1458
Wiesenfeld BM (1997) Procedural unfairness and managers surviving layoffs: self-threat and coping in the context of organizational change. Soc Justice Res 10(2):225–239
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Additional information
This research was carried out while the author was funded by the Irish Research Council for the Humanities and Social Sciences. I am indebted to Julia Anna Matz, Ron Davies, Liam Delaney, Kanika Kapur, Oliver Morrissey, Mark McGovern, Peter Neary, Karl Whelan, two anonymous referees and the editor for helpful comments and suggestions.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Gillanders, R. Corruption and anxiety in Sub-Saharan Africa. Econ Gov 17, 47–69 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10101-015-0177-6
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10101-015-0177-6