Abstract
The ability to recognize conspecifics by their acoustic signals is of crucial importance to social animals, especially where visibility is limited, because it allows for discrimination between familiar and unfamiliar individuals and facilitates associations with and the avoidance of particular conspecifics. Animals may also benefit from an ability to recognize and use the information coded into the auditory signals of other species. Companion species such as dogs, cats, and horses are able to discriminate between familiar and unfamiliar human voices; however, whether this ability is widespread across vertebrates is still unknown. Using playback experiments, we tested whether western gorillas living at Zoo Atlanta were able to discriminate between the voices of subgroups of people: i.e., unfamiliar individuals, familiar individuals with whom the gorillas had positive interactions, and familiar individuals with whom they had negative interactions. Gorillas responded significantly more often (longer gazing duration, higher gazing frequency, shorter latency, and larger number of distress behaviors) to the voices of unfamiliar and familiar-negative individuals than to those of familiar-positive individuals, indicating that they recognized the voices of subgroup of people based on familiarity and possibly the nature of the relationship with them. Future studies should determine whether this is also the case in the wild, where interspecific associations with humans are less intense than they are in captive settings.
Similar content being viewed by others
References
Adachi I, Kuwahata H, Fujita K (2007) Dogs recall their owner’s face upon hearing the owner’s voice. Anim Cogn 10:17–21. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-006-0025-8
Bakeman R (2005) Recommended effect size statistics for repeated measures designs. Behav Res Methods 37:379–384
Bates LA, Sayialel KN, Njiraini NW, Moss CJ, Poole JH, Byrne RW (2007) Elephants classify human ethnic groups by odor and garment color. Curr Biol 17:1938–1942. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2007.09.060
Boinski S, Campbell AF (1996) The huh vocalization of white-faced capuchins: a spacing call disguised as a food call? Ethology 102:826–840
Bouchet H, Pellier AS, Blois-Heulin C, Lemasson A (2010) Sex differences in the vocal repertoire of adult red-capped mangabeys (Cercocebus torquatus): a multi-level acoustic analysis. Am J Primatol 72:360–375. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20791
Candiotti A, Zuberbuehler K, Lemasson A (2013) Voice discrimination in four primates. Behav Process 99:67–72. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2013.06.010
Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM (1996) Function and intention in the calls of non-human primates. Proc Br Acad Lond B Biol Sci 88:59–76
Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM (1980) Vocal recognition in free-ranging vervet monkeys. Anim Behav 28:362–367
Cohen J (1988) Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences, 2nd edn. Erlbaum, Hillsdale
Crockford C, Herbinger I, Vigilant L, Boesch C (2004) Wild chimpanzees produce group-specific calls: a case for vocal learning? Ethology 110:221–243
Digweed SM, Fedigan LM, Rendall D (2007) Who cares who calls? Selective responses to the lost calls of socially dominant group members in the white-faced capuchin (Cebus capucinus). Am J Primatol 69:829–835
Doran-Sheehy DM, Derby AM, Greer D, Mongo P (2007) Habituation of western gorillas: the process and factors that influence it. Am J Primatol 69:1354–1369. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20442|ISSN0275-2565
Dorey N, Conover A, Udell M (2014) Interspecific communication from people to horses (Equus ferus caballus) is influenced by different horsemanship training styles. J Comp Psychol (washington, DC: 1983). https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037255
Driscoll CA, Macdonald DW, O’Brien SJ (2009) From wild animals to domestic pets, an evolutionary view of domestication. PNAS 106(Suppl. 1):9971–9978
Evans TA, Howell S, Westergaard GC (2005) Auditory-visual cross-modal perception of communicative stimuli in Tufted Capuchin Monkeys (Cebus apella). J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 31(4):399–406. https://doi.org/10.1037/0097-7403.31.4.399
Ey E, Hammerschmidt K, Seyfarth RM, Fischer J (2007a) Age- and sex-related variations in clear calls of Papio ursinus. Int J Primatol 28:947–960. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-007-9139-3
Ey E, Pfefferle D, Fischer J (2007b) Do age- and sex-related variations reliably reflect body size in non-human primate vocalizations? A review. Primates 48:253–267
Fallow P, Magrath R (2010) Eavesdropping on other species: mutual interspecific understanding of urgency information in avian alarm calls. Anim Behav 79:411–417. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.11.018
Fichtel C (2004) Reciprocal recognition of sifaka (Propithecus verreauxi verreauxi) and redfronted lemur (Eulemur fulvus rufus) alarm calls. Anim Cogn 7:45–52. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-003-0180-0
Ghazanfar AA, Logothetis NK (2003) Facial expressions linked to monkey calls. Nature 423:937–938. https://doi.org/10.1038/423937a
Griffin AS (2004) Social learning about predators: a review and prospectus. Anim Learn Behav 32:131–140. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196014
Hare B, Brown M, Williamson C, Tomasello M (2002) The domestication of social cognition in dogs. Science 298:1634–1636. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1072702
Hare B, Rosati A, Kaminski J, Bräuer J, Call J, Tomasello M (2010) The domestication hypothesis for dogs’ skills with human communication: a response to Udell et al. (2008) and Wynne et al. (2008). Anim Behav 79:e1–e6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.06.031
Hauser MD (1988) How infant vervet monkeys learn to recognize starling alarm calls: the role of experience. Behaviour 105:187–201. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853988x00016
Hedwig D, Mundry R, Robbins MM, Boesch C (2015) Audience effects, but not environmental influences, explain variation in gorilla close distance vocalizations—a test of the acoustic adaptation hypothesis. Am J Primatol 77:1239–1252. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.22462
Herbinger I, Papworth S, Boesch C, Zuberbühler K (2009) Vocal, gestural and locomotor responses of wild chimpanzees to familiar and unfamiliar intruders: a playback study. Anim Behav 78:1389–1396. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.09.010
Hohmann G, Fruth B (1994) Structure and use of distance calls in wild bonobos (Pan paniscus). Int J Primatol 15:767–782
Krebs JR, Dawkins R (1984) Animal signals: mind-reading and manipulation. In: Krebs JR, Davies NB (eds) Behavioural ecology: an evolutionary approach, 2nd edn. Sinauer, Sunderland, pp 380–402
Kriengwatana B, Escudero P, ten Cate C (2015) Revisiting vocal perception in non-human animals: a review of vowel discrimination, speaker voice recognition, and speaker normalization. Front Psychol. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01543
Lampe JF, Andre J (2012) Cross-modal recognition of human individuals in domestic horses (Equus caballus). Anim Cogn 15:623–630. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-012-0490-1
Lee WY, Han Y-D, Lee S-I, Jablonski PG, Jung J-W, Kim J-H (2016) Antarctic skuas recognize individual humans. Anim Cogn 19:861–865. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-016-0970-9
Lemasson A, Palombit RA, Jubin R (2008) Friendships between males and lactating females in a free-ranging group of olive baboons (Papio hamadryas anubis): evidence from playback experiments. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 62:1027–1035. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-007-0530-z
Lemasson A, Remeuf K, Rossard A, Zimmermann E (2012) Cross-taxa similarities in affect-induced changes of vocal behavior and voice in arboreal monkeys. PLoS ONE 7:e45106. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0045106
Leroux M, Hetem RS, Hausberger M, Lemasson A (2018) Cheetahs discriminate familiar and unfamiliar human voices. Sci Rep 8:15516. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-33971-1
Levey DJ et al (2009) Urban mockingbirds quickly learn to identify individual humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci 106:8959–8962. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0811422106
Levrero F, Mathevon N (2013) Vocal signature in wild infant chimpanzees. Am J Primatol 75:324–332. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.22108
Magrath RD, Pitcher BJ, Gardner JL (2009) Recognition of other species’ aerial alarm calls: speaking the same language or learning another? Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 276:769–774. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2008.1368
Marzluff JM, Walls J, Cornell HN, Withey JC, Craig DP (2010) Lasting recognition of threatening people by wild American crows. Anim Behav 79:699–707. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.12.022
McComb K, Shannon G, Sayialel KN, Moss C (2014) Elephants can determine ethnicity, gender, and age from acoustic cues in human voices. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 111:5433–5438. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1321543111
Meise K, Franks DW, Bro-Jorgensen J (2018) Multiple adaptive and non-adaptive processes determine responsiveness to heterospecific alarm calls in African savannah herbivores. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2017.2676
Mitani JC, Nishida T (1993) Contexts and social correlates of long-distance calling by male chimpanzees. Anim Behav 45:735–746
Muller CA, Manser MB (2008) The information banded mongooses extract from heterospecific alarms. Anim Behav 75:897–904. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.07.012
Olejnik S, Algina J (2003) Generalized Eta and Omega squared statistics: measures of effect size for some common research designs. Psychol Methods 8:434–447. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.8.4.434
Ogden JJ, Finlay TW, Maple TL (1990) Gorilla adaptations to naturalistic environments. Zoo Biol 9:107–121. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.1430090205
Palombit RA, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (1997) The adaptive value of “friendships” to female baboons: experimental and observational evidence. Anim Behav 54:599–614
Papworth S, Milner-Gulland EJ, Slocombe K (2013) Hunted Woolly Monkeys (Lagothrix poeppigii) show threat-sensitive responses to human presence. PLoS ONE. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0062000
Pfefferle D, Fischer J (2006) Sounds and size: identification of acoustic variables that reflect body size in hamadryas baboons, Papio Hamadryas. Anim Behav 72:43–51
Pougnault L, Levréro F, Mulot B, Lemasson A (2020) Breaking conversational rules matters to captive gorillas: A playback experiment. Sci Rep 10:6947. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-63923-7
Proops L, McComb K (2012) Cross-modal individual recognition in domestic horses (Equus caballus) extends to familiar humans. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 279:3131–3138. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2012.0626
Raemaekers JJ, Raemaekers PM (1985) Field playback of loud calls to gibbons (Hylobates lar)—territorial, sex-specific and species-specific responses. Anim Behav 33:481–493
Rainey HJ, Zuberbühler K, Slater PJB (2004) Hornbills can distinguish between primate alarm calls. Proc R Soc Lond Ser B Biol Sci 271:755–759
Ratcliffe VF, McComb K, Reby D (2014) Cross-modal discrimination of human gender by domestic dogs. Anim Behav 91:127–135. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.03.009
Rendall D, Rodman PS, Emond RE (1996) Vocal recognition of individuals and kin in free-ranging rhesus monkeys. Anim Behav 51:1007–1015
Rendall D, Owren MJ, Weerts E, Hienz RD (2004) Sex differences in the acoustic structure of vowel-like grunt vocalizations in baboons and their perceptual discrimination by baboon listeners. J Acoust Soc Am 115:411–421. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.1635838
Saito A, Shinozuka K (2013) Vocal recognition of owners by domestic cats (Felis catus). Anim Cogn 16:685–690. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-013-0620-4
Salmi R, Doran-Sheehy DM (2014) The function of loud calls (Hoot series) in wild western gorillas (Gorilla gorilla). Am J Phys Anthropol 155:379–391. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.22575
Salmi R, Hammerschmidt K, Doran-Sheehy DM (2013) Western gorilla vocal repertoire and contextual use of vocalizations. Ethology 119:831–847. https://doi.org/10.1111/eth.12122
Savage-Rumbaugh S, Sevcik RA, Hopkins WD (1988) Symbolic cross-modal transfer in two species of chimpanzees. Child Dev 59:617–625
Schmidt KA, Dall SRX, van Gils JA (2010) The ecology of information: an overview on the ecological significance of making informed decisions. Oikos 119:304–316. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0706.2009.17573.x
Seiler M, Schwitzer C, Gamba M, Holderied MW (2013) Interspecific semantic alarm call recognition in the solitary Sahamalaza sportive lemur, Lepilemur sahamalazensis. PLoS ONE 8:e67397. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0067397
Seppänen J-T, Forsman JT, Monkkonen M, Thomson RL (2007) Social information use is a process across time, space, and ecology, reaching heterospecifics. Ecology 88:1622–1633. https://doi.org/10.1890/06-1757.1
Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (1980a) The ontogeny of vervet monkey alarm calling behavior: a preliminary report. Zeitschrift Fuer Tierpsychologie 54:37–56
Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (1980b) Vocal reocgnition in free ranging vervet monkeys. Anim Behav 28:362–367
Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (1990) The assessment by vervet monkeys of their own and another species’ alarm calls. Anim Behav 40:754–764
Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (2008) The evolution of social categories. In: Bethoz A, Christen Y (eds) Neurobiology of “umwelt”: how living beings perceive the world. Springer, Berlin, pp 69–87
Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (2012) Social relationships, social cognition, and the evolution of mind in primates. In: Handbook of psychology, 2nd edn. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118133880.hop203021
Shriner WM (1998) Yellow-bellied marmot and golden-mantled ground squirrel responses to heterospecific alarm calls. Anim Behav 55:529–536. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1997.0623
Sliwa J, Duhamel J-R, Pascalis O, Wirth S (2011) Spontaneous voice-face identity matching by rhesus monkeys for familiar conspecifics and humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 108:1735–1740. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1008169108
Stephan C, Wilkinson A, Huber L (2012) Have we met before? Pigeons recognise familiar human faces. Avian Biol Res 5:75–80. https://doi.org/10.3184/175815512x13350970204867
Struhsaker TT (1981) Polyspecific associations among tropical rain-forest primates. Z Tierpsychol 57:268–304. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1981.tb01928.x
Tomczak A, Tomczak E (2014) The need to report effect size estimates revisited. An overview of some recommended measures of effect size. Trends Sport Sci 1:19–25
Uster D, Zuberbühler K (2001) The functional significance of Diana monkey “clear” calls. Behaviour 138:741–756
Ward P, Zahavi A (1973) The importance of certain assemblages of birds as ‘Information-Centres’ for food finding. Ibis 115:517–534
Wascher CAF, Szipl G, Boeckle M, Wilkinson A (2012) You sound familiar: carrion crows can differentiate between the calls of known and unknown heterospecifics. Anim Cogn 15:1015–1019. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-012-0508-8
Waser PM (1975) Experimental playbacks show vocal mediation of intergroup avoidance in a forest monkey. Nature 255:56–58
Weiss DJ, Garibaldi BT, Hauser MD (2001) The production and perception of long calls by cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus): acoustic analyses and playback experiments. J Comp Psychol 115:258–271
Wich SA, Assink PR, Becher F, Sterck EHM (2002) Playbacks of loud calls to wild Thomas langurs (Primates; Presbytis thomasi): the effect of familiarity. Behaviour 139:79–87. https://doi.org/10.1163/15685390252902292
Wich SA, Sterck EHM (2007) Familiarity and threat of opponents determine variation in Thomas langur (Presbytis thomasi) male behaviour during between-group encounters. Behaviour 144:1583–1598. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853907782512065
Zayan R, Vauclair J (1998) Categories as paradigms for comparative cognition. Behav Proc 42:87–99. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0376-6357(97)00064-8
Zuberbühler K (2000) Interspecies semantic communication in two forest primates. Proc R Soc Lond Ser B Biol Sci 267:713–718
Acknowledgements
We thank all the Zoo Atlanta workers (caregivers, veterinarians, and maintenance staff) and UGA employees who agreed to have their voices recorded. We are particularly grateful to the gorilla caregivers who helped us logistically with this project, and to the gorillas who participated in this study. We thank Haleigh Randazzo, who co-coded the videos, and the Center for Geospatial Research for the use of video equipment. The project was funded by the University of Georgia and through The Experiment Crowdsource Platform. We would like to personally thank Douglas Murray, Alex Piel, Ari Grossman, Manuela Nanni, Barbara Orelli, Lynne LaVallee, David, Ashley Sullivan, Stacy Watts, Marc Myers, Wendy M. Erb, Eileen Larney, Theresa R. Jones, Debbie Copan, Edwin Watts, Jade Schmitt, Tara Stoinski, Cristiano Marinucci, Jessica Lodwick, S. R. Batchelder, Cindy Maupin, Cornelia Seiffert, Patti L. R., Kerry Ossi-Lupo, Stacy Eason, Cara N. Love, Donita Schultz, John Boone, Natalie Schwob, Alexis Stern, Christina Doyle Sheehan, Linda Fabiani, Cherri Cherri, Jan Gogarten, Anja Deppe, Raymond Vagell, Fabrizio Salmi, Eric D. Walters, and a few more anonymous supporters for their donations. Our research protocol was approved by the Zoo Atlanta Scientific Review and by the Committee Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of the University of Georgia and adhered to the Code of Best Practices for Field Primatology of the International Primatological Society and the American Society of Primatology.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Additional information
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Salmi, R., Jones, C.E. & Carrigan, J. Who is there? Captive western gorillas distinguish human voices based on familiarity and nature of previous interactions. Anim Cogn 25, 217–228 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-021-01543-y
Received:
Revised:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-021-01543-y