Advertisement

Animal Cognition

, Volume 20, Issue 5, pp 953–960 | Cite as

Element repetition rates encode functionally distinct information in pied babbler ‘clucks’ and ‘purrs’

  • Sabrina Engesser
  • Amanda R. Ridley
  • Simon W. Townsend
Original Paper

Abstract

Human language is a recombinant system that achieves its productivity through the combination of a limited set of sounds. Research investigating the evolutionary origin of this generative capacity has generally focused on the capacity of non-human animals to combine different types of discrete sounds to encode new meaning, with less emphasis on meaning-differentiating mechanisms achieved through potentially simpler temporal modifications within a sequence of repeated sounds. Here we show that pied babblers (Turdoides bicolor) generate two functionally distinct vocalisations composed of the same sound type, which can only be distinguished by the number of repeated elements. Specifically, babblers produce extended ‘purrs’ composed of, on average, around 17 element repetitions when drawing young offspring to a food source and truncated ‘clucks’ composed of a fixed number of 2–3 elements when collectively mediating imminent changes in foraging site. We propose that meaning-differentiating temporal structuring might be a much more widespread combinatorial mechanism than currently recognised and is likely of particular value for species with limited vocal repertoires in order to increase their communicative output.

Keywords

Element repetition Temporal structure Animal communication Combinatoriality Language evolution Turdoides bicolor 

Notes

Acknowledgements

We thank the Kalahari Research Trust, T. Clutton-Brock, M. Manser, the de Bruins and Kotzes for logistics and access to land; researchers and assistants at the Pied Babbler Project; R. Mundry for providing the R-pDFA script; M. Manser for support; and M. Manser, C. Bousquet, K. Bard and two anonymous reviewers for comments on previous versions.

Funding

Funding was provided by the Forschungskredit of the University of Zurich Grants 57191601 (to SE) and FK-14-077 (to SE), Swiss National Science Foundation Grants P1ZHP3_151648 (to SE), 31003A_153065 (to SWT), and PP00P3_163850 (to SWT), and the Claraz Stiftung (to SWT).

Author contributions

SE, ARR and SWT designed research; SE performed research and analysed data; SE and SWT wrote paper.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethics statement

Permission was provided by the ethical committee for animal research of the University of Cape Town and the Northern Cape Conservation Authority, South Africa.

Supplementary material

10071_2017_1114_MOESM1_ESM.xlsx (26 kb)
Supplementary material 1 (XLSX 25 kb)
10071_2017_1114_MOESM2_ESM.pdf (54 kb)
Supplementary material 2 (PDF 53 kb)

References

  1. Antunes R, Schulz T, Gero S, Whitehead H, Gordon J, Rendell L (2011) Individually distinctive acoustic features in sperm whale codas. Anim Behav 81:723–730. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.12.019 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Barto’n K (2014) MuMIn: multi-model inference. R package version 1.10.5. http://cran.r-project.org/package=MuMIn. Accessed 29 Apr 2016
  3. Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker B, Walker S (2015) Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J Stat Softw 67:1–48. doi: 10.18637/jss.v067.i01 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bell MBV, Radford AN, Rose R, Wade HM, Ridley AR (2009) The value of constant surveillance in a risky environment. Proc R Soc B 276:2997–3005. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2009.0276 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  5. Blumstein DT (2007) The evolution, function, and meaning of marmot alarm communication. Adv Stud Behav 37:371–401. doi: 10.1016/s0065-3454(07)37008-3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bohn KM, Schmidt-French B, Ma ST, Pollak GD (2008) Syllable acoustics, temporal patterns, and call composition vary with behavioral context in Mexican free-tailed bats. J Acoust Soc Am 124:1838–1848. doi: 10.1121/1.2953314 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  7. Bousquet CA, Sumpter DJ, Manser MB (2011) Moving calls: a vocal mechanism underlying quorum decisions in cohesive groups. Proc R Soc B 278:1482–1488. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.1739 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Burnham KP, Anderson DR (2002) Model selection and multimodel inference: a practical information-theoretic approach, 2nd edn. Springer, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  9. Collier K, Bickel B, van Schaik CP, Manser MB, Townsend SW (2014) Language evolution: syntax before phonology? Proc R Soc B 281:20140263. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2014.0263 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  10. DuBois AL, Nowicki S, Searcy WA (2009) Swamp sparrows modulate vocal performance in an aggressive context. Biol Lett 5:163–165. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2008.0626 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Engesser S, Ridley AR, Townsend SW (2016) Meaningful call combinations and compositional processing in the southern pied babbler. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 113:5976–5981. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1600970113 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  12. Filzmoser P, Garrett RG, Reimann C (2005) Multivariate outlier detection in exploration geochemistry. Comput Geosci 31:579–587. doi: 10.1016/j.cageo.2004.11.013 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Fox J, Weisberg W (2011) An R companion to applied regression, 2nd edn. Sage, Thousand Oaks CAGoogle Scholar
  14. Frank AF (2011) R-hacks/mer-utils.R. https://github.com/aufrank/R-hacks/blob/master/mer-utils.R. Accessed 29 April 2016
  15. Golabek KA (2010) Vocal communication and the facilitation of social behaviour in the southern pied babbler (Turdoides bicolor). Dissertation, University of BristolGoogle Scholar
  16. Halekoh U, Højsgaard S (2014) A kenward-roger approximation and parametric bootstrap methods for tests in linear mixed models—the R package pbkrtest. J Stat Softw 59:1–32. doi: 10.18637/jss.v059.i09 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Hollén LI, Bell MBV, Radford AN (2008) Cooperative sentinel calling? Foragers gain increased biomass intake. Curr Biol 18:576–579. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.02.078 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Hollén LI, Bell MBV, Russell A, Niven F, Ridley AR, Radford AN (2011) Calling by concluding sentinels: coordinating cooperation or revealing risk? PLoS ONE 6:e25010. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0025010 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  19. Humphries D (2012) The mechanisms and function of social recognition in the cooperatively breeding Southern pied babbler, Turdoides bicolor. Dissertation, Macquarie UniversityGoogle Scholar
  20. Humphries D, Finch FM, Bell MB, Ridley AR (2016) Vocal cues to identity: pied babblers produce individually distinct but not stable loud calls. Ethology 122:609–619. doi: 10.1111/eth.12508 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Janik V, Slater P (1997) Vocal learning in mammals. Adv Stud Behav 26:59–99. doi: 10.1016/s0065-3454(08)60377-0 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Kershenbaum A, Blumstein DT, Roch MA et al (2014) Acoustic sequences in non-human animals: a tutorial review and prospectus. Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc 91:13–52. doi: 10.1111/brv.12160 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  23. Marler P (1977) The structure of animal communication sounds. In: Recognition of complex acoustic signals: report of Dahlem workshop. Abakon Verlagsgesellschaft, BerlinGoogle Scholar
  24. Mundry R, Sommer C (2007) Discriminant function analysis with nonindependent data: consequences and an alternative. Anim Behav 74:965–976. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.12.028 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Radford AN, Ridley AR (2006) Recruitment calling: a novel form of extended parental care in an altricial species. Curr Biol 16:1700–1704. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.06.053 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Radford AN, Ridley AR (2007) Individuals in foraging groups may use vocal cues when assessing their need for anti-predator vigilance. Biol Lett 3:249–252. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2007.0110 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  27. Radford AN, Ridley AR (2008) Close calling regulates spacing between foraging competitors in the group-living pied babbler. Anim Behav 75:519–527. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.05.016 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Raihani NJ, Ridley AR (2007a) Adult vocalizations during provisioning: offspring response and postfledging benefits in wild pied babblers. Anim Behav 74:1303–1309. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.02.025 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Raihani NJ, Ridley AR (2007b) Variable fledging age according to group size: trade-offs in a cooperatively breeding bird. Biol Lett 3:624–627. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2007.0435 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  30. Raihani NJ, Ridley AR (2008) Experimental evidence for teaching in wild pied babblers. Anim Behav 75:3–11. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.07.024 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. R-Core-Team (2014) R: a language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, ViennaGoogle Scholar
  32. Rek P, Osiejuk TS (2012) Temporal patterns of broadcast calls in the corncrake encode information arbitrarily. Behav Ecol 24:547–552. doi: 10.1093/beheco/ars196 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Ridley AR, Wiley EM, Thompson AM (2014) The ecological benefits of interceptive eavesdropping. Funct Ecol 28:197–205. doi: 10.1111/1365-2435.12153 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Schel AM, Tranquilli S, Zuberbühler K (2009) The alarm call system of two species of black-and-white colobus monkeys (Colobus polykomos and Colobus guereza). J Comp Psychol 123:136–150. doi: 10.1037/a0014280 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Schel AM, Candiotti A, Zuberbühler K (2010) Predator-deterring alarm call sequences in Guereza colobus monkeys are meaningful to conspecifics. Anim Behav 80:799–808. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.07.012 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Sharp SP, McGowan A, Wood MJ, Hatchwell BJ (2005) Learned kin recognition cues in a social bird. Nature 434:1127–1130. doi: 10.1038/nature03522 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Sueur C, Deneubourg JL (2011) Self-organization in primates: understanding the rules underlying collective movements. Int J Primatol 32:1413–1432. doi: 10.1007/s10764-011-9520-0 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Thompson A (2012) Post-fledging begging and development in Southern Pied Babblers (Turdoides bicolor). Dissertation, University of Cape TownGoogle Scholar
  39. Venables WN, Ripley BD (2002) Modern applied statistics with S, 4th edn. Springer, New YorkCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Wheatcroft D (2015) Repetition rate of calls used in multiple contexts communicates presence of predators to nestlings and adult birds. Anim Behav 103:35–44. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.02.009 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Zuberbühler K (2015) Linguistic capacity of non-human animals. WIREs Cogn Sci 6:313–321. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1338 CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Animal Behaviour, Department of Evolutionary Biology and Environmental StudiesUniversity of ZurichZurichSwitzerland
  2. 2.Pied Babbler Research ProjectKuruman River ReserveSouth Africa
  3. 3.Centre for Evolutionary Biology, School of Animal BiologyThe University of Western AustraliaCrawleyAustralia
  4. 4.Percy FitzPatrick InstituteUniversity of Cape TownRondeboschSouth Africa
  5. 5.Department of PsychologyUniversity of WarwickCoventryUK
  6. 6.Department of Comparative LinguisticsUniversity of ZurichZurichSwitzerland
  7. 7.Department of Comparative LinguisticsUniversity of ZurichZurichSwitzerland

Personalised recommendations