Animal Cognition

, Volume 20, Issue 4, pp 717–723

Differences in problem-solving between canid populations: Do domestication and lifetime experience affect persistence?

Original Paper

Abstract

Past research has suggested that a variety of factors, phylogenetic and ontogenetic, play a role in how canines behave during problem-solving tasks and the degree to which the presence of a human influences their problem-solving behaviour. While comparisons between socialized wolves and domestic dogs have commonly been used to tease apart these predictive factors, in many cases a single dog population, often pets, have been used for these comparisons. Less is understood about how different populations of dogs may behave when compared with wolves, or with each other, during an independent problem-solving task. This experiment compared the independent persistence of four populations of canines (two groups of pet domestic dogs, a group of free-ranging domestic dogs, and human-socialized wolves) on an independent problem-solving task in the presence of an on looking human. Results showed that wolves persisted the most at the task while free-ranging dogs persisted the least. Free-ranging dogs gazed at the human experimenter for the longest durations during the task. While further research is needed to understand why these differences exist, this study demonstrates that dogs, even those living outside human homes as scavengers, show comparatively low levels of persistence when confronted with a solvable task in the presence of a human as well as significantly greater duration of human-directed gaze when compared with wolves.

Keywords

Canines Problem-solving Persistence Dogs 

References

  1. Benson-Amram S, Holekamp KE (2012) Innovative problem solving by wild spotted hyenas. Proc R Soc B 279(1744):4087–4095. doi:10.1098/rspb.2012.1450 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  2. Bentosela M, Barrera G, Jakovcevic A, Elgier A, Mustaca A (2008) Effect of reinforcement, reinforcer omission and extinction on a communicative response in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris). Behav Process 78(3):464–469. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2008.03.004 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bentosela M, Wynne CDL, D’Orazio M, Elgier A, Udell MAR (2016) Sociability and gazing toward humans in dogs and wolves: simple behaviours with broad implications. J Exp Anal Behav 105(1):68–75. doi:10.1002/jeab.191 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Bhadra A, Bhattacharjee D, Paul M, Singh A, Gade PR, Shrestha P, Bhadra A (2015) The meat of the matter: a rule of thumb for scavenging dogs? Ethol Ecol Evol 28(4):1–14. doi:10.1080/03949370.2015.1076526 Google Scholar
  5. Chow PKY, Lea SEG, Leaver LA (2016) How practice makes perfect: the role of persistence, flexibility and learning in problem-solving efficiency. Anim Behav 112:273–283. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.11.014 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Coppinger R, Coppinger L, Beck AM (2016) What is a dog?. The University of Chicago Press, ChicagoCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. D’Aniello B, Scandurra A (2016) Ontogenetic effects on gazing behaviour: a case study of kennel dogs (Labrador Retrievers) in the impossible task paradigm. Anim Cogn 19(3):565–570. doi:10.1007/s10071-016-0958-5 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. D’Aniello B, Scandurra A, Prato-Previde E, Valsecchi P (2015) Gazing toward humans: a study on water rescue dogs using the impossible task paradigm. Behav Process 110:68–73. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2014.09.022 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Fiset S, Plourde V (2013) Object permanence in domestic dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) and gray wolves (Canis lupus). J Comp Psychol 127(2):115–127. doi:10.1037/a0030595 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Frank H, Frank MG (1982) Comparison of problem-solving performance in six-week-old wolves and dogs. Anim Behav 30(1):95–98. doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(82)80241-8 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Gácsi M, Gyori B, Virányi Z, Kubinyi E, Range F, Belényi B, Miklósi A (2009) Explaining dog wolf differences in utilizing human pointing gestures: selection for synergistic shifts in the development of some social skills. PLoS ONE. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0006584 PubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Hall NJ, Lord K, Arnold A-MK, Wynne CDL, Udell MAR (2015) Assessment of attachment behaviour to human caregivers in wolf pups (Canis lupus lupus). Behav Process 110:15–21. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2014.11.005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Jakovcevic A, Elgier AM, Mustaca AE, Bentosela M (2010) Breed differences in dogs’ (Canis familiaris) gaze to the human face. Behav Process 84(2):602–607. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2010.04.003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Klinghammer E, Goodman P (1987) Socialization and management of wolves in captivity. In: Frank H (ed) Man and wolf: advances, issues, and problems in captive wolf research. Kluwer Academic Publishers, Dordrecht, pp 31–61Google Scholar
  15. Lazarowski L, Dorman DC (2015) A comparison of pet and purpose-bred research dog (Canis familiaris) performance on human-guided object-choice tasks. Behav Process 110:60–67. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2014.09.021 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Marshall-Pescini S, Passalacqua C, Barnard S, Valsecchi P, Prato-Previde E (2009) Agility and search and rescue training differently affects pet dogs’ behaviour in socio-cognitive tasks. Behav Process 81(3):416–422. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2009.03.015 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Marshall-Pescini S, Colombo E, Passalacqua C, Merola I, Prato-Previde E (2013) Gaze alternation in dogs and toddlers in an unsolveable task: evidence of an audience effect. Anim Cogn 16:933–943. doi:10.1007/s10071-013-0627-x CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Mech LD (2012) The wolf: the ecology and behaviour of an endangered species. Natural History Press, Garden CityGoogle Scholar
  19. Miklósi Á, Kubinyi E, Topál J, Gácsi M, Virányi Z, Csányi V (2003) A simple reason for a big difference: wolves do not look back at humans, but dogs do. Curr Biol 13(9):763–766. doi:10.1016/S0960-9822(03)00263-X CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Nagasawa M, Mitsui S, En S, Ohtani N, Ohta M, Sakuma Y, Onaka T, Mogi K, Kikusui T (2015) Oxytocin-gaze positive loop and the coevolution of human-dog bonds. Science 348(6232):333–336. doi:10.1126/science.1261022 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Ortolani A, Vernooij H, Coppinger R (2009) Ethiopian village dogs: behavioural responses to a stranger’s approach. Appl Anim Behav Sci 119(3–4):210–218. doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2009.03.011 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Passalacqua C, Marshall-Pescini S, Barnard S, Lakatos G, Valsecchi P, Prato Previde E (2011) Human-directed gazing behaviour in puppies and adult dogs, Canis lupus familiaris. Anim Behav 82(5):1043–1050. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.07.039 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Paul M, Majumder SS, Nandi A, Bhadra A (2015) Selfish mothers indeed! resource-dependent conflict over extended parental care in free-ranging dogs. R Soc Open Sci 2(12):150580. doi:10.1098/rsos.150580 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  24. Range F, Möslinger H, Virányi Z (2012) Domestication has not affected the understanding of means-end connections in dogs. Anim Cogn 15(4):597–607. doi:10.1007/s10071-012-0488-8 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  25. Scandurra A, Prato-Previde E, Valsecchi P, Aria M, D’Aniello B (2015) Guide dogs as a model for investigating the effect of life experience and training on gazing behaviour. Anim Cogn 18(4):937–944. doi:10.1007/s10071-015-0864-2 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Thornton A, Samson J (2012) Innovative problem solving in wild meerkats. Anim Behav 83(6):1459–1468. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.03.018 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Udell MAR (2015) When dogs look back: inhibition of independent problem-solving behaviour in domestic dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) compared with wolves (Canis lupus). Biol Lett 11(9):20150489. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2015.0489 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  28. Udell MAR, Wynne CDL (2010) Ontogeny and phylogeny: both are essential to human-sensitive behaviour in the genus Canis. Anim Behav 79(2):e9–e14. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.11.033 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Udell MAR, Dorey NR, Wynne CDL (2008) Wolves outperform dogs in following human social cues. Anim Behav 76(6):1767–1773. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.07.028 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Udell MAR, Dorey NR, Wynne CDL (2009) What did domestication do to dogs? a new account of dogs’ sensitivity to human actions. Biol Rev 85(2):327–345. doi:10.1111/j.1469-185X.2009.00104.x CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Udell MAR, Dorey NR, Wynne CDL (2010) The performance of stray dogs (Canis familiaris) living in a shelter on human-guided object-choice tasks. Anim Behav 79(3):717–725. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.12.027 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Udell MAR, Dorey NR, Wynne CDL (2011) Can your dog read your mind? understanding the causes of canine perspective taking. Learn Behav 39(4):289–302. doi:10.3758/s13420-011-0034-6 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Udell MAR, Ewald M, Dorey NR, Wynne CDL (2014) Exploring breed differences in dogs: does exaggeration or inhibition of predatory response predict human-guided task performance? Anim Behav 89:99–105. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.12.012 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Virányi Z, Gácsi M, Kubinyi E, Topál J, Belényi B, Ujfalussy D, Miklósi Á (2008) Comprehension of human pointing gestures in young human-reared wolves (Canis lupus) and dogs (Canis familiaris). Anim Cogn 11(3):373–387. doi:10.1007/s10071-007-0127-y CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Animal and Rangeland SciencesOregon State UniversityCorvallisUSA
  2. 2.Behaviour and Ecology Lab, Department of Biological SciencesIndian Institute of Science Education and Research – KolkataMohanpurIndia

Personalised recommendations