Animal Cognition

, Volume 19, Issue 5, pp 867–878 | Cite as

Neurobehavioral evidence for individual differences in canine cognitive control: an awake fMRI study

Original Paper

Abstract

Based on behavioral evidence, the domestic dog has emerged as a promising comparative model of human self-control. However, while research on human inhibition has probed heterogeneity and neuropathology through an integration of neural and behavioral evidence, there are no parallel data exploring the brain mechanisms involved in canine inhibition. Here, using a combination of cognitive testing and awake neuroimaging in domestic dogs, we provide evidence precisely localizing frontal brain regions underpinning response inhibition in this species and demonstrate the dynamic relationship between these regions and behavioral measures of control. Thirteen dogs took part in an in-scanner go/no-go task and an out-of-scanner A-not-B test. A frontal brain region was identified showing elevated neural activity for all subjects during successful inhibition in the scanner, and dogs showing greater mean brain activation in this region produced fewer false alarms. Better performance in the go/no-go task was also correlated with fewer errors in the out-of-scanner A-not-B test, suggesting that dogs show consistent neurobehavioral individual differences in cognitive control, as is seen in humans. These findings help establish parity between human and canine mechanisms of self-control and pave the way for future comparative studies examining their function and dysfunction.

Keywords

Self-control Motor inhibition Prefrontal cortex Individual differences Dog cognition Comparative cognition fMRI Neuroimaging 

Supplementary material

Supplementary Video 1: Go-No-Go:

This video demonstrates successful neutral, no-go and go trials, as well as a typical false alarm response on a no-go trial. Here the dog (“Callie”) is performing the task in her custom-made chin rest in a training context. (MP4 6475 kb)

Supplementary Video 2: A-Not-B:

This video demonstrates the test phase of the A-not-B procedure with one dog (“Stella”). After three trials of familiarization to location A, the dog is tested in her ability to switch to location B. (MP4 18519 kb)

References

  1. Adams B, Chan A, Callahan H, Milgram NW (2000) The canine as a model of human cognitive aging: recent developments. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 24:675–692CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Amici F, Aureli F, Call J (2008) Fission–fusion dynamics, behavioral flexibility, and inhibitory control in primates. Curr Biol 18:1415–1419CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Andics A, Gácsi M, Faragó T, Kis A, Miklósi Á (2014) Voice-sensitive regions in the dog and human brain are revealed by comparative fMRI. Curr Biol 24:574–578CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Andrews Espy K, Kaufmann P, McDiarmid M, Glisky M (1999) Executive functioning in preschool children: performance on A-not-B and other delayed response format tasks. Brain Cogn 41:178–199CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Aron A, Durston S, Eagle D, Logan G, Stinear C, Stuphorn V (2007) Converging evidence for a fronto-basal-ganglia network for inhibitory control of action and cognition. J Neurosci 27:11860–11864CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Aron A, Robbins T, Poldrack R (2014) Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex: one decade on. Trends Cogn Sci 18:177–185CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Artelle KA, Dumoulin LK, Reimchen TE (2011) Behavioural responses of dogs to asymmetrical tail wagging of a robotic dog replica. Laterality 16:129–135PubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Avants BB, Tustison NJ, Song G, Cook PA, Klein A, Gee JC (2011) A reproducible evaluation of ANTs similarity metric performance in brain image registration. NeuroImage 54:2033–2044CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Barkley RA (1997) Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychol Bull 121:65CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Baum CF (2008) Stata tip 63: modeling proportions. Stata J 8:299Google Scholar
  11. Berns GS, Brooks AM, Spivak M (2012) Functional MRI in awake unrestrained dogs. PLoS ONE 7:e38027CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  12. Berns GS, Brooks AM, Spivak M (2014) Scent of the familiar: an fMRI study of canine brain responses to familiar and unfamiliar human and dog odors. Behav Process 110:37–46CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Bray E, MacLean E, Hare B (2014) Context specificity of inhibitory control in dogs. Anim Cogn 17:15–31CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Bray EE, MacLean EL, Hare BA (2015) Increasing arousal enhances inhibitory control in calm but not excitable dogs. Anim Cogn 18:1317–1329CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Brutkowski S, Dabrowska J (1963) Disinhibition after prefrontal lesions as a function of duration of intertrial intervals. Science 139:505–506CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Brutkowski S, Dabrowska J (1966) Prefrontal cortex control of differentiation behavior in dogs. Acta Biol Exp 26:425–439Google Scholar
  17. Brutrowski S, Mempel E (1961) Disinhibition of inhibitory conditioned responses following selective brain lesions in dogs. Science 134:2040–2041CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Casey B, Castellanos F, Giedd J, Marsh W, Hamburger S, Schubert A, Vauss Y, Vaituzis A, Dickstein D, Sarfatti S, Rapoport J (1997) Implication of right frontostriatal circuitry in response inhibition and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 36:374–383CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Churchill NW, Oder A, Abdi H, Tam F, Lee W, Thomas C et al (2012) Optimizing preprocessing and analysis pipelines for single-subject fMRI. I. Standard temporal motion and physiological noise correction methods. Hum Brain Map 33:609–627CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Clarke RS, Heron W, Fetherstonaugh ML, Forgays DG, Hebb DO (1951) Individual differences in dogs: preliminary report on the effects of early experience. Can J Psychol 5(4):150–156CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Cook PF, Spivak M, Berns GS (2014) One pair of hands is not like another: caudate BOLD response in dogs depends on signal source and canine temperament. PeerJ 2:e596CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  22. Cook PF, Brooks AM, Spivak M, Berns GS (2015) Regional brain activations in awake unrestrained dogs. J Vet Behav 10:440CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Cox RW (1996) AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput Biomed Res 29:162–173CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Dabrowska J, Szafranska-Kosmal A (1972) Partial prefrontal lesions and go-no go symmetrically reinforced differentiation test in dogs. Acta Neurobiol Exp 32:817–834Google Scholar
  25. Dale AM (1999) Optimal experimental design for event-related fMRI. Hum Brain Map 8:109–114CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Daskalakis ZJ, Christensen BK, Chen R, Fitzgerald PB, Zipursky RB, Kapur S (2002) Evidence for impaired cortical inhibition in schizophrenia using transcranial magnetic stimulation. Arch Gen Psychiatry 59:347–354CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Datta R, Lee J, Duda J, Avants B, Vite C, Tseng B, Gee J, Aguirre G, Aguirre G (2012) A digital atlas of the dog brain. PLoS ONE. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0052140 Google Scholar
  28. Dilks DD, Cook P, Weiller SK, Berns HP, Spivak MH, Berns G (2015) Awake fMRI reveals a specialized region in dog temporal cortex for face processing. Peer J 3:e1115. https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.1115
  29. Eagle D, Bari A, Robbins T (2008) The neuropsychopharmacology of action inhibition: cross-species translation of the stop-signal and go/no-go tasks. Psychopharmacology 199:439–456CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Farah MJ (1994) Neuropsychological inference with an interactive brain: a critique of the locality assumption. Behav Brain Sci 17:43–61CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Forstmann BU, Jahfari S, Scholte HS, Wolfensteller U, van den Wildenberg WP, Ridderinkhof KR (2008) Function and structure of the right inferior frontal cortex predict individual differences in response inhibition: a model-based approach. J Neurosci 28(39):9790–9796CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Friston KJ, Ashburner J, Frith CD, Poline JB, Heather JD, Frackowiak RS (1995) Spatial registration and normalization of images. Hum Brain Map 3:165–189CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Gibbons JD, Chakraborti S (2011) Nonparametric statistical inference. Springer, Berlin, pp 977–979Google Scholar
  34. Goldstein RZ, Volkow ND (2002) Drug addiction and its underlying neurobiological basis: neuroimaging evidence for the involvement of the frontal cortex. Am J Psychiatry 159:1642–1652CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  35. Guo K, Meints K, Hall C, Hall S, Mills D (2009) Left gaze bias in humans, rhesus monkeys and domestic dogs. Anim Cogn 12:409–418CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Hagler DJ, Saygin AP, Sereno MI (2006) Smoothing and cluster thresholding for cortical surface-based group analysis of fMRI data. Neuroimage 33:1093–1103CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  37. Horn NR, Dolan M, Elliott R, Deakin JFW, Woodruff PWR (2003) Response inhibition and impulsivity: an fMRI study. Neuropsychologia 41:1959–1966CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Jia H, Pustovyy O, Waggoner P, Beyers R, Schumacher J, Wildey C, Barrett J, Morrison E, Salibi N, Denney T, Vodyanoy V, Deshpande G (2014) Functional MRI of the olfactory system in conscious dogs. Plos ONE. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0086362 Google Scholar
  39. Jones AC, Gosling SD (2005) Temperament and personality in dogs (Canis familiaris): a review and evaluation of past research. Appl Anim Behav Sci 95:1–53CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Kanai R, Rees G (2011) The structural basis of inter-individual differences in human behaviour and cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci 12:231–242CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Kane M, Engle R (2002) The role of prefrontal cortex in working-memory capacity, executive attention, and general fluid intelligence: an individual differences perspective. Pyschon B Rev 9:637–671CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Kosmal A, Markow G, Stepniewska I (1984) The presylvian cortex as a transitional prefronto-motor zone in dogs. Acta Neurbiol 44:273–287Google Scholar
  43. Logan G, Schachar R, Tannock R (1997) Impulsivity and inhibitory control. Psychol Sci 8:60–64CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Logothetis N, Pauls J, Augath M, Trinath T, Oeltermann A (2001) Neurphysiological investigation of the basis of the fMRI signal. Nature 412:150–157CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Long JS (1997) Regression models for categorical and limited dependent variables. Sage, LondonGoogle Scholar
  46. MacLean E, Hare B, Nunn C, Addessi E, Amici F, Anderson R et al (2014) The evolution of self control. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 111:E2140–E2148. doi:10.1073/pnas.1323533111 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  47. Miller H, Pattison K, DeWall C, Rayburn-Reeves R, Zentall T (2010) Self-control without a “self”? Psychol Sci 21:534–538CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Miller H, DeWall C, Pattison K, Molet M, Zentall T (2012) Too dog tired to avoid danger: self-control depletion in canines increases behavioral approach toward an aggressive threat. Psychon B Rev 19:535–540CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Miyake A, Friedman N, Emerson M, Witzki A, Howerter A, Wager T (2000) The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex frontal lobe tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn Psychol 41:49–1000CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Nichols T, Hayasaka S (2003) Controlling the familywise error rate in functional neuroimaging: a comparative review. Stat Methods Med Res 12:419–446CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. Osthaus B, Marlow D, Ducat P (2010) Minding the gap: spatial perseveration error in dogs. Anim Cogn 12:881–885CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Overall KL (2000) Natural animal models of human psychiatric conditions: assessment of mechanism and validity. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 24:727–776CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Peremans K, Audenaert K, Coopman F, Blanckaert P, Jacobs F, Otte A et al (2003) Estimates of regional cerebral blood flow and 5-HT2A receptor density in impulsive, aggressive dogs with 99mTc-ECD and 123I-5-I-R91150. Eur J Nucl Med Mol I 30:1538–1546CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Piaget J (1954) The construction of reality in the child. Basic Books, New YorkCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Pongrácz P, Miklósi Á, Kubinyi E, Topál J, Csányi V (2003) Interaction between individual experience and social learning in dogs. Anim Behav 65:595–603CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Power JD, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE (2015) Recent progress and outstanding issues in motion correction in resting state fMRI. Neuroimage 105:536–551CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  57. Poyser F, Caldwell C, Cobb M (2006) Dog paw preference shows lability and sex differences. Behav Proc 73:216–221CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Preuss T, Goldman-Rakic P (1991) Myelo- and cytoarchitecture of the granular frontal cortex and surrounding regions in the strepsirhine primate galago and the anthropoid primate macaca. J Comp Neurol 310:429–474CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  59. Quaranta A, Siniscalchi M, Vallortigara G (2007) Asymmetric tail-wagging responses by dogs to different emotive stimuli. Curr Biol 17:199–201CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Riemer S, Mills D, Wright H (2014) Impulsive for life? The nature of long-term impulsivity in domestic dogs. Anim Cogn 17:815–819CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  61. Rubia K, Russell T, Overmeyer S, Brammer M, Bullmore E, Sharma T, Simmons A, Williams S, Giampietro V, Andrew C, Taylor E (2000) Mapping motor inhibition: conjunctive brain activations across different versions of Go/No-Go and stop tasks. Neuroimage 13:250–261CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Sagvolden T, Russell V, Aase H, Johansen E, Farshbaf M (2005) Rodent models of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry 57:1239–1247CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  63. Schoenbaum G, Nugent S, Saddoris M, Setlow B (2002) Orbitofrontal lesions in rats impair reversal but not acquisition of go, no-go odor discriminations. NeuroReport 13:885–890CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  64. Sharp D, Bonnelle V, De Boissezon X, Beckmann C, James S, Patel M, Mehta M (2010) Distinct frontal systems for response inhibition, attentional capture, and error processing. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 107:6106–6111CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  65. Siniscalchi M, Lusito R, Vallortigara G, Quaranta A (2013) Seeing left-or right-asymmetric tail wagging produces different emotional responses in dogs. Curr Biol 23:2279–2282CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  66. Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Woolrich MW, Beckmann CF, Behrens TE, Johansen-Berg H et al (2004) Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage 23:S208–S219CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  67. Stępień I, Stępień L, Kreiner J (1963) The effects of total and partial ablations of the premotor cortex on the instrumental conditioned reflexes in dogs. Acta Biol Exp 23:45–59Google Scholar
  68. Stepniewska I, Kosmal A (1986) Distribution of mediodorsal thalamic nucleus afferents originating in the prefrontal association cortex of the dog. Acta Neurobiol 46:311–322Google Scholar
  69. Tanaka D (1987) Neostriatal projections from cytoarchitectonically defined gyri in the prefrontal cortex of the dog. J Comp Neurol 261:48–73CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  70. Tapp PD, Siwak CT, Estrada J, Head E, Muggenburg BA, Cotman CW, Milgram NW (2003) Size and reversal learning in the beagle dog as a measure of executive function and inhibitory control in aging. Learn Mem 10:64–73CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  71. Thompson PM, Schwartz C, Lin RT, Khan AA, Toga AW (1996) Three-dimensional statistical analysis of sulcal variability in the human brain. J Neurosci 16:4261–4274PubMedGoogle Scholar
  72. Tomkins LM, McGreevy PD, Branson NJ (2010) Lack of standardization in reporting motor laterality in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris). J Vet Behav 5:235–239CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Topál J, Gergely G, Erdőhegyi Á, Csibra G, Miklósi Á (2009) Differential sensitivity to human communication in dogs, wolves, and human infants. Science 325:1269–1272CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  74. Van den Berg L, Schilder MBH, Knol BW (2003) Behavior genetics of canine aggression: behavioral phenotyping of golden retrievers by means of an aggression test. Behav Genet 33:469–483CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  75. Wayne RK (1986) Cranial morphology of domestic and wild canids: the influence of development on morphological change. Evolution 40:243–261CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Wells DL (2003) Lateralised behaviour in the domestic dog, Canis familiaris. Behav Process 61:27–35CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Wobber V, Hare B, Koler-Matznick J, Wrangham R, Tomasello M (2009) Breed differences in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) comprehension of human communicative signals. Interact Stud 10:206–224CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Wright HF, Mills DS, Pollux PM (2012) Behavioural and physiological correlates of impulsivity in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris). Physiol Behav 105:676–682CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyEmory UniversityAtlantaUSA
  2. 2.Comprehensive Pet TherapySandy SpringsUSA

Personalised recommendations