Animal Cognition

, Volume 18, Issue 1, pp 165–178 | Cite as

Individual differences in learning speed, performance accuracy and exploratory behaviour in black-capped chickadees

  • Lauren M. Guillette
  • Allison H. Hahn
  • Marisa Hoeschele
  • Ann-Marie Przyslupski
  • Christopher B. Sturdy
Original Paper

Abstract

Cognitive processes are important to animals because they not only influence how animals acquire, store and recall information, but also may underpin behaviours such as deciding where to look for food, build a nest, or with whom to mate. Several recent studies have begun to examine the potential interaction between variation in cognition and variation in personality traits. One hypothesis proposed that there is a speed–accuracy trade-off in cognition ability that aligns with a fast–slow behaviour type. Here, we explicitly examined this hypothesis by testing wild-caught black-capped chickadees in a series of cognitive tasks that assessed both learning speed (the number of trials taken to learn) and accuracy (post-acquisition performance when tested with un-trained exemplars). Chickadees’ exploration scores were measured in a novel environment task. We found that slow-exploring chickadees demonstrated higher accuracy during the test phase, but did not learn the initial task in fewer trials compared to fast-exploring chickadees, providing partial support for the proposed link between cognition and personality. We report positive correlations in learning speed between different phases within cognitive tasks, but not between the three cognitive tasks suggesting independence in underlying cognitive processing. We discuss different rule-based strategies that may contribute to differential performance accuracy in cognitive tasks and provide suggestions for future experimentation to examine mechanisms underlying the relationship between cognition and personality.

Keywords

Animal personality Black-capped chickadee Cognition Exploratory behaviour Individual differences Learning 

Supplementary material

This video is a successful trial (pass) in the Colour association task. Yellow was the S+, and blue was the S- (MPG 8512 kb)

This video is an unsuccessful trial (fail) in the Colour association task. Red was the S+, and green was the S- (MPG 5976 kb)

This video is a successful trial (pass) in the Colour association task. Yellow was the S+, and blue was the S- (MPG 18054 kb)

This video is an unsuccessful trial (fail) with the transparent tube in the Detour-reaching task (MPG 1788 kb)

This video is a successful trial (pass) with the transparent tube in the Detour-reaching task (MPG 1070 kb)

References

  1. Amy M, van Oers K, Naguib M (2012) Worms under cover: relationships between performance in learning tasks and personality in great tits (Parus major). Anim Cogn 5:763–770. doi:10.1007/s10071-012-0500-3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bell AM, Foster SA, Wund M (2013) Evolutionary perspectives on personality in stickleback fish. In: Carere C, Maestripieri D (eds) Animal personalities: behavior physiology and evolution. The University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 36–65Google Scholar
  3. Biro PA, Dingemanse NJ (2010) Sampling bias resulting from animal personality. Trends Ecol Evol 24:66–67. doi:10.1016/j.tree.2008.10.004 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bloomfield TM (1967) A peak shift on a line tilt continuum. J Exp Anal Behav 4:361–366CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Boogert NJ, Reader S, Laland KN (2006) The relation between social rank, neophobia and individual learning in starlings. Anim Behav 1229–1239. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.02.021
  6. Boogert NJ, Giraldeau L-A, Lefebvre L (2008) Song complexity correlates with learning ability in zebra finch males. Anim Behav 76:1735–1741. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.08.009 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Boogert NJ, Anderson RC, Peters S et al (2011) Song repertoire size in male song sparrows correlates with detour reaching, but not with other cognitive measures. Anim Behav 81:1209–1216. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.03.004 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bouchard J, Goodyer W, Lefebvre L (2007) Social learning and innovation are positively correlated in pigeons (Columba livia). Anim Cogn 10:259–266. doi:10.1007/s10071-006-0064-1 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Brust V, Wuerz Y, Krüger O (2013) Behavioural flexibility and personality in zebra finches. Ethology 119. doi:10.1111/eth.12095
  10. Budaev SV, Zhuikov AY (1998) Avoidance learning and “personality” in the guppy (Poecilia reticulata). J Comp Psychol 112:92–94CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Carere C, Locurto C (2011) Interaction between animal personality and animal cognition. Curr Zool 57:491–498Google Scholar
  12. Carere C, Caramaschi D, Fawcett TW (2010) Covariation between personalities and individual differences in coping with stress: converging evidence and hypotheses. Curr Zool 56:728–740Google Scholar
  13. Charrier I, Bloomfield LL, Sturdy CB (2004) Note types and coding in parid vocalizations. I: the chick-a-dee call of the black-capped chickadee (Poecile atricapillus). Can J Zool 779:769–779. doi:10.1139/Z04-045 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Charrier I, Lee TT, Bloomfield LL, Sturdy CB (2005) Acoustic mechanisms of note-type perception in black-capped chickadee (Poecile atricapillus) calls. J Comp Psychol 119:371–380. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.119.4.371 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Chittka L, Skorupski P, Raine NE (2009) Speed-accuracy tradeoffs in animal decision making. Trends Ecol Evol 24:400–407. doi:10.1016/j.tree.2009.02.010 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Cockrem JF (2007) Stress, corticosterone responses and avian personalities. J für Ornithol 148:169–178. doi:10.1007/s10336-007-0175-8 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Cole EF, Quinn JL (2012) Personality and problem-solving performance explain competitive ability in the wild. Proc Biol Sci 279:1168–1175. doi:10.1098/rspb.2011.1539 PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Cole EF, Cram DL, Quinn JL (2011) Individual variation in spontaneous problem-solving performance among wild great tits. Anim Behav 81:491–498. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.11.025 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Cole EF, Morand-Ferron J, Hinks AE, Quinn JL (2012) Cognitive ability influences reproductive life history variation in the wild. Curr Biol 22:1808–1812. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2012.07.051 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Coppens CM, de Boer SF, Koolhaas JM (2010) Coping styles and behavioural flexibility: towards underlying mechanisms. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 365:4021–4028. doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0217 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Dingemanse NJ, de Goede P (2004) The relation between dominance and exploratory behavior is context-dependent in wild great tits. Behav Ecol 15:1023–1030. doi:10.1093/beheco/arh115 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Dingemanse NJ, Both C, van Noordwijk AJ et al (2003) Natal dispersal and personalities in great tits (Parus major). Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 270:741–747. doi:10.1098/rspb.2002.2300 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Dingemanse NJ, Both C, Drent PJ, Tinbergen JM (2004) Fitness consequences of avian personalities in a fluctuating environment. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 271:847–852. doi:10.1098/rspb.2004.2680 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Drent PJ, van Oers K, Van Noordwijk AJ (2003) Realized heritability of personalities in the great tit (Parus major). Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 270:45–51. doi:10.1098/rspb.2002.2168 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Dugatkin LA, Alfieri MS (2003) Boldness, behavioral inhibition and learning. Ethol Ecol Evol 15:43–49CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Exnerová A, Svádová KH, Fucíková E et al (2010) Personality matters: individual variation in reactions of naive bird predators to aposematic prey. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 723–728. doi:10.1098/rspb.2009.1673
  27. Federation of Alberta Naturalists (2007) Black-capped chickadee (Poecile atricapillus). Atlas breed. Birds Alberta a second look. Federation of Alberta Naturalists, Altona, pp 387–391Google Scholar
  28. Galsworthy MJ, Paya-Cano JL, Liu L et al (2005) Assessing reliability, heritability and general cognitive ability in a battery of cognitive tasks for laboratory mice. Behav Genet 35:675–692. doi:10.1007/s10519-005-3423-9 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Groothuis TGG, Carere C (2005) Avian personalities: characterization and epigenesis. Biobehav Rev 29:137–150. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.06.010 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Guenther A, Brust V, Dersen M, Trillmich F (2014) Learning and personality types are related in cavies (Cavia aperea). J Comp Psychol 128:74–81. doi:10.1037/a0033678 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Guillette LM, Reddon AR, Hurd PL, Sturdy CB (2009) Exploration of a novel space is associated with individual differences in learning speed in black-capped chickadees, Poecile atricapillus. Behav Process 82:265–270. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2009.07.005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Guillette LM, Bailey AA, Reddon AR (2010a) A brief report: capture order is repeatable in chickadees. Int J Comp Psychol 9:216–224Google Scholar
  33. Guillette LM, Farrell TM, Hoeschele M et al (2010b) Mechanisms of call note-type perception in black-capped chickadees (Poecile atricapillus): peak shift in a note-type continuum. J Comp Psychol 124:109–115. doi:10.1037/a0017741 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Guillette LM, Reddon AR, Hoeschele M, Sturdy CB (2011) Sometimes slower is better: slow-exploring birds are more sensitive to changes in a vocal discrimination task. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 278:767–773. doi:10.1098/rspb.2010.1669 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Hanson HM (1959) Effects of discrimination training on stimulus generalization. J Exp Psychol 58:321–334PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Hollander FA, Van Overveld T, Tokka I, Matthysen E (2008) Personality and nest defence in the great tit (Parus major). Ethology 114:405–412. doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.2008.01488.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Honig WK, Urcuioli PJ (1981) The legacy of Guttman and Kalish: 25 years of reearch on stimulus generalization. J Exp Anal Behav 3:405–445CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Honig WK, Boneau CA, Burstein KR, Pennypacker HS (1963) Positive and negative generalization gradients obtained after equivalent training conditions. J Comp Physiol Psychol 56:111–116. doi:10.1037/h0048683 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Keagy J, Savard J-F, Borgia G (2011) Complex relationship between multiple measures of cognitive ability and male mating success in satin bowerbirds, Ptilonorhynchus violaceus. Anim Behav 81:1063–1070. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.02.018 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Koolhaas JM, Korte SM, De Boer SF et al (1999) Coping styles in animals: current status in behavior and stress-physiology. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 23:925–935PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Koolhaas JM, de Boer SF, Coppens CM, Buwalda B (2010) Neuroendocrinology of coping styles: towards understanding the biology of individual variation. Front Neuroendocrinol 31:307–321. doi:10.1016/j.yfrne.2010.04.001 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Marchetti C, Drent PJ (2000) Individual differences in the use of social information in foraging by captive great tits. Anim Behav 131–140. doi:10.1006/anbe.2000.1443
  43. Matzel LD, Han YR, Grossman H et al (2003) Individual differences in the expression of a “general” learning ability in mice. J Neurosci 23:6423–6433PubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Naguib M, Kazek A, Schaper SV et al (2010) Singing activity reveals personality traits in great tits. Ethology 116:763–769. doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.2010.01791.x Google Scholar
  45. Nakagawa S, Gillespie DOS, Hatchwell BJ, Burke T (2007) Predictable males and unpredictable females: sex difference in repeatability of parental care in a wild bird population. J Evol Biol 20:1674–1681. doi:10.1111/j.1420-9101.2007.01403.x PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Niemelä PT, Vainikka A, Forsman JT et al (2013) How does variation in the environment and individual cognition explain the existence of consistent behavioral differences? Ecol Evol 3:457–464. doi:10.1002/ece3.451 PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Pyle P (1997) Identification guide to North American birds. Part 1. Slate Creek Press, BolinasGoogle Scholar
  48. Reale D, Reader S, Sol D et al (2007) Integrating animal temperament within ecology and evolution. Biol Rev 82:291–318. doi:10.1111/j.1469-185X.2007.00010.x PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Reale D, Dingemanse NJ, Kazem AJ, Wright J (2010) Evolutionary and ecological approaches to the study of personality. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 365:3937–3946. doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0222 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Schuett W, Dall SRX (2009) Sex differences, social context and personality in zebra finches, Taeniopygia guttata. Anim Behav 77:1041–1050. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.12.024 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Sih A, Del Giudice M (2012) Linking behavioural syndromes and cognition: a behavioural ecology perspective. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 367:2762–2772. doi:10.1098/rstb.2012.0216 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Smith SM (1991) The black-capped chickadee: behavioral ecology and natural history. Comstock/Cornell University Press, IthicaGoogle Scholar
  53. Sneddon LU (2003) The bold and the shy: individual differences in rainbow trout. J Fish Biol 44:971–975. doi:10.1046/j.1095-8649.2003.00084.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Sturdy CB, Phillmore LS, Weisman RG (2000) Call-note discriminations in black-capped chickadees (Poecile atricapillus). J Comp Psychol 114:357–364PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Sturdy CB, Bloomfield LL, Farrell TM et al (2007) Auditory category perception as a natural cognitive activity in songbirds. Comp Cogn Behav Rev 2:93–110Google Scholar
  56. Tebbich S, Stankewitz S, Teschke I (2011) The relationship between foraging, learning abilities and neophobia in two species of Darwin’s finches. Ethology 135–146. doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.2011.02001.x
  57. Thornton A, Lukas D (2012) Individual variation in cognitive performance: developmental and evolutionary perspectives. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 367:2773–2783. doi:10.1098/rstb.2012.0214 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Titulaer M, van Oers K, Naguib M (2012) Personality affects learning performance in difficult tasks in a sex-dependent way. Anim Behav 83:723–730. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.12.020 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Van Oers K, Naguib M (2013) Avian personality. In: Carere C, Maestripieri D (eds) Animal personal behavior physiology evolution. The University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 66–95CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Van Oers K, Drent PJ, Dingemanse NJ, Kempenaers B (2008) Personality is associated with extrapair paternity in great tits, Parus major. Anim Behav 555–563. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.03.011
  61. Verbeek MEM, Drent PJ, Wiepkema PR (1994) Consistent individual differences in early exploratory behaviour in male great tits. Anim Behav 48:1113–1121CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Vlamings PHJM, Hare B, Call J (2010) Reaching around barriers: the performance of the great apes and 3–5-year-old children. Anim Cogn 13:273–285. doi:10.1007/s10071-009-0265-5 PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  • Lauren M. Guillette
    • 1
    • 2
  • Allison H. Hahn
    • 1
  • Marisa Hoeschele
    • 1
    • 3
  • Ann-Marie Przyslupski
    • 1
    • 4
  • Christopher B. Sturdy
    • 1
    • 5
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of AlbertaEdmontonCanada
  2. 2.School of Biology, Harold Mitchel BuildingUniversity of St AndrewsSt AndrewsUK
  3. 3.Department of Cognitive BiologyUniversity of ViennaViennaAustria
  4. 4.Department of PaediatricsUniversity of AlbertaEdmontonCanada
  5. 5.Centre for NeuroscienceUniversity of AlbertaEdmontonCanada

Personalised recommendations