Skip to main content
Log in

Transitive inference by pigeons: Does the geometric presentation of the stimuli make a difference?

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Animal Cognition Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In studies of transitive inference (TI), nonhuman animals are typically trained with the following 5-term task: A+B−, B+C−, C+D−, D+E− where the letters stand for arbitrary stimuli and [+] indicates that choice is reinforced and [−] indicates that choice is not reinforced. A TI effect is found when, given the untrained test pair BD, subjects choose B. TI effects have been found in many nonhuman species. Although reinforcement history has been posited as an account of the TI effect, it has failed to account for a variety of conditions under which TI effects have been found. A more cognitive account of TI is that organisms are able to form a representation of the series (A > B > C > D > E). In support of this hypothesis, Roberts and Phelps (Psychol Sci 5:368–374, 1994) found that presentation of the pairs of stimuli in a linear arrangement facilitated TI performance by rats, whereas presentation of the pairs of stimuli in a circular arrangement did not. Using methods adapted from Roberts and Phelps, we trained pigeons on either a linear or a circular arrangement of stimuli with the 5-term task. Results indicated that on the BD test pair, pigeons trained with a circular arrangement did not differ from those trained with a linear arrangement. Furthermore, we found that memory for training pairs was variable and was highly correlated with degree of TI. The results suggest that regardless of how pigeons are able to represent the stimuli, choice was not affected by the spatial arrangement of the stimuli during training.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Acuna BD, Eliassen JC, Donoghue JP, Sanes JN (2002) Frontal and parietal lobe activation during transitive inference in humans. Cereb Cortex 12:1312–1321

    Google Scholar 

  • Benard J, Giurfa M (2004) A test of transitive inferences in free-flying honeybees: unsuccessful performance due to memory constraints. Learn Mem 11:328–336

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bond AB, Wei CA, Kamil AC (2010) Cognitive representation in transitive inference: a comparison of four corvid species. Behav Process 85:283–292

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boysen ST, Berntson GG, Shreyer TA, Quigley KS (1993) Processing of ordinality and transitivity by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J Comp Psychol 107:208–215

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bryant PE, Trabasso T (1971) Transitive inferences and memory in young children. Nature 232:456–458

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bunsey M, Eichenbaum H (1996) Conservation of hippocampal memory function in rats and humans. Nature 379:255–257

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen J, Bussey K (2003) Rats form cognitive maps from spatial configurations of proximal arm cues in an enclosed 4-arm radial maze. Learn Motiv 34:168–184

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Daniels CW, Laude JR, Zentall TR. Six-term transitive inference with pigeons: successive-pair training followed by mixed-pair training. J Exp Anal Behav. doi:10.1002/jeab.65

  • Davis H (1992) Transitive inference in rats (Rattus norvegicus). J Comp Psychol 106:342–349

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Soto CB, London M, Handel S (1965) Social reasoning and spatial paralogic. J Pers Soc Psychol 2:513–521

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DeVito LM, Kanter BR, Eichenbaum H (2010) The hippocampus contributes to memory expression during transitive inference in mice. Hippocampus 20:208–217

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dickinson A (1980) Contemporary animal learning theory. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Fersen LV, Wynne CDL, Delius JD, Staddon JER (1991) Transitive inference formation in pigeons. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 17:334–341

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallistel CR (1990) The organization of learning. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Gazes RP, Chee NW, Hampton RR (2012) Cognitive mechanisms for transitive inference performance in rhesus monkeys: measuring the influence of associative strength and inferred order. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 38:331–345

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gillan DJ (1981) Reasoning in the chimpanzee: II. Transitive inference. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 7:150–164

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grosenick L, Clement TS, Fernald RD (2007) Fish can infer social rank by observation alone. Nature 445:429–432

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Halford GS, Kelly ME (1984) On the basis of early transitivity judgments. J Exp Child Psychol 38:42–63

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Higa JJ, Staddon JER (1993) “Transitive inference” in multiple conditional discriminations. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 59:265–291

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kallio KD (1982) Developmental change on a five-term transitive inference. J Exp Child Psychol 33:142–164

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kelly DM, Spetch ML, Heth CD (1998) Pigeons’(Columba livia) encoding of geometric and featural properties of a spatial environment. J Comp Psychol 112:259–269

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lazareva OF, Smirnova AA, Bagozkaja MS, Zorina ZA, Rayevsky VV, Wasserman EA (2004) Transitive responding in hooded crows requires linearly ordered stimuli. J Exp Anal Behav 82:1–19. doi:10.1901/jeab.2004.82-1

    Google Scholar 

  • Lazareva OF, Wasserman EA (2006) Effects of stimulus orderability and reinforcement history on transitive responding in pigeons. Behav Process 76:161–172

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lazareva OF, Wasserman EA (2010) Nonverbal transitive inference: effects of task and awareness on human performance. Behav Process 83:99–112

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lazareva OF, Wasserman EA (2012) Transitive inference in pigeons: measuring the associative values of stimuli B and D. Behav Process 8:244–255

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Libben M, Titone D (2008) The role of awareness and working memory in human transitive inference. Behav Process 77:43–54

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • McGonigle BO, Chalmers M (1977) Are monkeys logical? Nature 267:694–696

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Olson DJ (1991) Species differences in spatial memory among Clark’s nutcrackers, scrub jays, and pigeons. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 17:363–376

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget J (1952) Judgment and reasoning in the child. The Humanities Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Roberts WA, Phelps MT (1994) Transitive inference in rats: a test of the spatial coding hypothesis. Psychol Sci 5:368–374

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roberts WA, Van Veldhuizen N (1985) Spatial memory in pigeons on the radial maze. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 11:241

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Singer RA, Abroms BD, Zentall TR (2006) Formation of a simple cognitive map by rats. Int J Comp Psychol 19:417–425

    Google Scholar 

  • Spetch ML, Mondloch MV (1993) Control of pigeons’ spatial search by graphic landmarks in a touch-screen task. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 19:353–372

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steirn JN, Weaver JE, Zentall TR (1995) Transitive inference in pigeons: simplified procedures and a test of value transfer theory. Learn Behav 23:76–82

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Terrace HS (1986) A nonverbal organism’s knowledge of ordinal position in a serial learning task. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 12:203

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trabasso T, Riley CA (1975) The construction and use of representations involving linear order. In: Solso RL (ed) Information processing and cognition: The Loyola symposium. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp 381–410

    Google Scholar 

  • Treichler FR, Van Tilburg D (1996) Concurrent conditional discrimination tests of transitive inference by macaque monkeys: list linking. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 22:105–117

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Urcuioli PJ, Michalek S (2007) Value transfer contributes to ambiguous-cue discrimination learning. Psychon Bull Rev 14:658–662

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vasconcelos M (2008) Transitive inference in non-human animals: an empirical and theoretical analysis. Behav Process 78:313–334

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weaver JE, Steirn JN, Zentall TR (1997) Transitive inference in pigeons: control for differential value transfer. Psychon Bull Rev 4:113–117

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wendelken C, Bunge SA (2009) Transitive inference: distinct contributions of rostrolateral prefrontal cortex and the hippocampus. J Cogn Neurosci 22:837–847

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woocher FD, Glass AL, Holyoak KJ (1978) Positional discriminability in linear orderings. Mem Cogn 6:165–173

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wynne CDL (1995) Reinforcement accounts for transitive inference performance. Anim Learn Behav 23:207–217

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wynne CDL (1998) A minimal model of transitive inference. In: Wynne CDL, Staddon JER (eds) Models of action: mechanisms for adaptive behavior. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, New Jersey, pp 269–307

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Thomas R. Zentall.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Daniels, C.W., Laude, J.R. & Zentall, T.R. Transitive inference by pigeons: Does the geometric presentation of the stimuli make a difference?. Anim Cogn 17, 973–981 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-014-0729-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-014-0729-0

Keywords

Navigation