Animal Cognition

, Volume 15, Issue 4, pp 737–743 | Cite as

Does the A-not-B error in adult pet dogs indicate sensitivity to human communication?

  • Anna Kis
  • József Topál
  • Márta Gácsi
  • Friederike Range
  • Ludwig Huber
  • Ádám Miklósi
  • Zsófia Virányi
Short Communication

Abstract

Recent dog–infant comparisons have indicated that the experimenter’s communicative signals in object hide-and-search tasks increase the probability of perseverative (A-not-B) errors in both species (Topál et al. 2009). These behaviourally similar results, however, might reflect different mechanisms in dogs and in children. Similar errors may occur if the motor response of retrieving the object during the A trials cannot be inhibited in the B trials or if the experimenter’s movements and signals toward the A hiding place in the B trials (‘sham-baiting’) distract the dogs’ attention. In order to test these hypotheses, we tested dogs similarly to Topál et al. (2009) but eliminated the motor search in the A trials and ‘sham-baiting’ in the B trials. We found that neither an inability to inhibit previously rewarded motor response nor insufficiencies in their working memory and/or attention skills can explain dogs’ erroneous choices. Further, we replicated the finding that dogs have a strong tendency to commit the A-not-B error after ostensive-communicative hiding and demonstrated the crucial effect of socio-communicative cues as the A-not-B error diminishes when location B is ostensively enhanced. These findings further support the hypothesis that the dogs’ A-not-B error may reflect a special sensitivity to human communicative cues. Such object-hiding and search tasks provide a typical case for how susceptibility to human social signals could (mis)lead domestic dogs.

Keywords

Dog A-not-B error Social cognition Communication 

Supplementary material

Supplementary material 1 (WMV 1752 kb)

Supplementary material 2 (WMV 1730 kb)

Supplementary material 3 (WMV 1812 kb)

Supplementary material 4 (WMV 1755 kb)

Supplementary material 5 (WMV 1748 kb)

References

  1. Csibra G, Gergely G (2009) Natural pedagogy. Trends Cogn Sci 13:148–153PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Diamond A (1985) Development of the ability to use recall to guide action, as indicated by infants’ performance on AB. Child Dev 56:868–883PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Erdőhegyi Á, Topál J, Viranyi Zs, Miklósi Á (2007) Dog-logic: inferential reasoning in a two-way choice task and its restricted use. Anim Behav 74:725–737CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Fiset S (2010) Comment on “differential sensitivity to human communication in dogs, wolves and human infants.” Science 329:142-bGoogle Scholar
  5. Gagnon S, Doré F (1994) A cross-sectional study of object permanence in domestic puppies (Canis familiaris). J Comp Psychol 108:220–232PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Gratch G, Appel KJ, Evans WF, LeCompte GK, Wright N (1974) Piaget’s stage IV object concept error: evidence of forgetting or object conception? Child Dev 45:71PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Hartshorne J (2008) Visual working memory capacity and proactive interference. PLoS One 3(7):e2716PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Kaminski J (2009) Dogs (Canis familiaris) are adapted to receive human communication. In: Berthoz A, Christen Y (eds) Neurobiology of umwelt: how living beings perceive the world. Springer Verlag, Berlin, pp 103–107Google Scholar
  9. Kis A, Gácsi M, Range F, Virányi Z (2012) Object permanence in adult common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus)—not everything is an “A-not-B” error that seems to be one. Anim Cogn 15(1):97−105Google Scholar
  10. Kupán K, Gy Gergely, Miklósi Á, Topál J (2011) Why do dogs (Canis familiaris) select the empty container in an observational learning task? Anim Cogn 14:259–268PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Longo MR, Bertenthal BI (2006) Common coding of observation and execution of action in 9-month-old infants. Infancy 10:43–59CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Marshall-Pescini S, Passalacqua C, Valsecchi P, Prato-Previde E (2010) Comment on “differential sensitivity to human communication in dogs, wolves and human infants.”Science 329:142-cGoogle Scholar
  13. Piaget J (1954) The construction of reality in the child. Basic Books, New YorkCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Smith L, Thelen E, Titzier R, McLin D (1999) Knowing in the context of acting: the task dynamics of the A-not-B error. Psychol Rev 106:235–260PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Somppi S, Törnqvist H, Hänninen L, Krause C, Vainio O (2011) Dogs do look at images—eye tracking in canine cognition research. Anim Cogn. doi:10.1007/s10071-011-0442-1 PubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Sümegi Zs (2011) Egy egyszerű hiba komplex magyarázata: a perszeverációt befolyásoló tényezők kutyáknál. Eötvös University, Master ThesisGoogle Scholar
  17. Tomasello M, Kaminski J (2009) Like infant, like dog. Science 325:1213–1214PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Topál J, Gy Gergely, Miklósi Á, Erdőhegyi Á, Csibra G (2008) Infants perseverative search errors are induced by pragmatic misinterpretation. Science 321:1831–1834PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Topál J, Gy Gergely, Erdőhegyi Á, Csibra G, Miklósi Á (2009) Differential sensitivity to human communication in dogs, wolves and human infants. Science 325:1269–1272PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Topál J, Miklósi Á, Sümegi Zs, Kis A (2010) Response to comments on “differential sensitivity to human communication in dogs, wolves and human infants.” Science 329:142-dGoogle Scholar
  21. Wellmann HM, Cross D, Bartsch K (1987) Infant search and object permanence: A meta-analysis of the A-not-B error. Mon Soc Res Child Dev 51:1–51Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2012

Authors and Affiliations

  • Anna Kis
    • 1
  • József Topál
    • 2
  • Márta Gácsi
    • 1
  • Friederike Range
    • 3
    • 4
  • Ludwig Huber
    • 3
    • 4
  • Ádám Miklósi
    • 1
  • Zsófia Virányi
    • 3
    • 4
  1. 1.Department of EthologyEötvös UniversityBudapestHungary
  2. 2.Research Centre for Natural Sciences, Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience and PsychologyHungarian Academy of SciencesBudapestHungary
  3. 3.Clever Dog LabViennaAustria
  4. 4.Department of Cognitive BiologyUniversity of ViennaViennaAustria

Personalised recommendations