Abstract
There is accumulating evidence that a variety of species possess quantitative abilities although their cognitive substrate is still unclear. This study is the first to investigate whether sea lions (Otaria flavescens), in the absence of training, are able to assess and select the larger of two sets of quantities. In Experiment 1, the two sets of quantities were presented simultaneously as whole sets, that is, the subjects could compare them directly. In Experiment 2, the two sets of quantities were presented item-by-item, and the totality of items was never visually available at the time of choice. For each type of presentation, we analysed the effect of the ratio between quantities, the difference between quantities and the total number of items presented. The results showed that (1) sea lions can make relative quantity judgments successfully and (2) there is a predominant influence of the ratio between quantities on the subjects’ performance. The latter supports the idea that an analogue representational mechanism is responsible for sea lions’ relative quantities judgments. These findings are consistent with previous reports of relative quantities judgments in other species such as monkeys and apes and suggest that sea lions might share a similar mechanism to compare and represent quantities.
Similar content being viewed by others
References
Acevedo J, Aguayo A, Sielfeld W (2003) Eventos reproductivos del león marino común (Otaria flavescens) (Shaw 1800), en el norte de Chile (Pacífico suroriental). Rev Biol Mar Oceanogr 38(2):69–75
Anderson US, Stoinski TS, Bloomsmith MA, Maple TL (2007) Relative numerousness judgment and summation in Young, middleaged, and older adult Orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus ablii and Pongo pygmaeus pygmaeus). J Comp Psychol 121:1–11
Baron R, Kenny DA (1986) The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J Pers Soc Psychol 61(6):1173–1182
Beran MJ (2001) Summation and numerousness judgments of sequentially presented sets of items by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J Comp Psychol 115:181–191
Beran MJ (2004) Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) respond to non-visible sets after one-by-one addition and removal of items. J Comp Psychol 118:25–36
Beran MJ (2007) Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) succeed on computerized test designed to assess conservation of discrete quantity. Anim Cogn 10:37–45
Botham MS, Kerfoot CJ, Louca V, Krause J (2005) Predator choice in the field; grouping guppies, Poecilia reticulate, receive more attacks. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 59:181–184
Brannon EM, Terrace HS (2000) Representation of the numerosities 1–9 by rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 26:31–49
Brannon EM, Terrace HS (2002) The evolution and ontogeny of ordinal numerical ability. In: Bekoff M, Allen C, Burghardt GM (eds) The cognitive animal: empirical and theoretical perspectives on animal cognition. The MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 197–204
Call J (2000) Estimating and operating on discrete quantities in orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus). J Comp Psychol 114:136–147
Cantlon JF, Brannon EM (2006) Shared system for ordering small and large numbers in monkeys and humans. Psychol Sci 17:401–406
Capozzo H (2002) South American sea lion (Otaria flavescens). In: Perrin W, Wursig B, Thewissen J (eds) Encyclopedia of marine mammals. Academic Press, New York, pp 1143–1146
Carey S (2001) Cognitive foundations of arithmetic: evolution and ontogenesis. Mind Lang 16:37–55
Davis H, Perusse R (1988) Numerical competence in animals: definitional issues, current evidence, and a new research agenda. Behav Brain Sci 11:561–615
Dehaene S (ed) (2001) The cognitive neuroscience of consciousness. MIT Press, Cambridge
Dooley GB, Gill TV (1977) Acquisition and use of mathematical by a linguistic chimpanzee. In: Rumbaugh DM (ed) Language learning by a chimpanzee: the Lana project. Academic Press, New York, pp 247–260
Evans T, Beran M, Harris E, Rice D (2009) Quantity judgments of sequentially presented food items by capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Anim Cogn 12:97–105
Gallistel CR, Gelman R (2000) Non-verbal numerical cognition: from reals and integers. Trends Cogn Sci 4:59–65
Genty E, Roeder J (2006) Self-control: why should sea lions (Zalophus californianus), perform better than primates? Anim Behav 72:1241–1247
Hanus D, Call J (2007) Discrete quantity judgments in the great apes (Pan paniscus, Pan troglodytes, Gorilla gorilla, Pongo pygmaeus): the effect of presenting whole sets versus item-by-item. J Comp Psychol 121:241–249
Hauser MD (2005) Our chimpanzee mind. Nature 437:60–63
Hauser MD, Carey S (1998) Building a cognitive creature from a set of primitives: evolutionary and developmental insights. In: Cummins D, Allen C (eds) The evolution of mind. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 51–106
Hauser MD, Spelke E (2004) Evolutionary and developmental foundations of human knowledge: a case study of mathematics. In: Gazzaniga MS (ed) The cognitive neurosciences III. The MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 853–864
Hauser MD, Carey S, Hauser LB (2000) Spontaneous number representation in semi-free-ranging rhesus monkeys. Proc Biol Sci 267(1445):829–833
Irie-Sugimoto N, Kobayashi T, Sato T, Hasegawa T (2009) Relative quantity judgment by Asian Elephants (Elephas maximus). Anim Cogn 12(1):193–199
Jaakkola K, Fellner W, Erb L, Rodriguez M, Guarino E (2005) Understanding of the concept of numerically “less” by bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). J Comp Psychol 119:286–303
Kahneman D, Treisman A (1984) Changing views of attention and automaticity. In: Parasuraman R, Davies D (eds) Varieties of attention. Academic Press, New York, pp 29–61
Kahneman D, Treisman A, Gibbs BJ (1992) The reviewing of object files: object-specific integration of information. Cogn Psychol 24:174–219
Kenny DA, Kashy DA, Bolger N (1998) Data analysis in social psychology. In: Gilbert D, Fiske S, Lindzey G (eds) The handbook of social psychology, vol 1, 4th edn. McGraw-Hill, Boston, pp 233–265
Kilian A, Yaman S, von Fersen L, Güntürkün O (2003) A bottlenose dolphin discriminates visual stimuli differing in numerosity. Learn Behav 31(2):133–142
Kitchen DM, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM (2004) Factors mediating inter-group encounters in savannah baboons (Papio cynocephalus ursinus). Behaviour 141:197–218
Libertus ME, Pruitt LB, Woldorff MG, Brannon EM (2009) Induced alpha-band oscillations reflect ratio-dependent number discrimination in the infant brain. J Cog Neurosci 21(12):2398–2406
MacKinnon DP, Warsi G, Dwyer JH (1995) A simulation study of mediated effect measures. Multivar Behav Res 30:41–62
McComb K, Packer C, Pusey A (1994) Roaring and numerical assessment in contests between groups of female lions (Panthera leo). Anim Behav 47:379–387
Meck WH, Church RM (1983) A mode control model of counting and timing processes. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 9:320–334
Mix K, Huttenlocher J, Levine S (2002) Multiple cues for quantification in infancy: is number one of them? Psychol Bull 128:278–294
Pack AA, Herman LM, Roitblat HL (1991) Generalization of visual matching and delayed matching by a California sea lion (Zalophus californianus). Anim Learn Behav 19:37–48
Reichmuth Kastak C, Schusterman RJ (2002) Long term memory for concepts in a California sea lion. Anim Cogn 5:225–232
Schusterman RJ, Reichmuth Kastak C, Kastak D (2002) The cognitive sea lion: meaning and memory in the lab and in nature. In: Bekoff M, Allen C, Burghardt G (eds) The cognitive animal: empirical and theoretical perspectives on animal cognition. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 217–228
Sepulveda M, Perez MJ, Sielfeld W, Oliva D, Duran LR, Rodriguez L, Araos V, Buscaglia M (2007) Operational interaction between South American sea lions (Otaria flavescens) and artisanal (small-scale) fishing in Chile: results from interview surveys and on-board observations. Fish Res 83(2–3):332–340
Shettleworth S (2009) The evolution of comparative cognition: is the snark still a boojum? Behav Process 80:210–217
Shettleworth S (2010) Cognition, evolution, and behaviour. Oxford University Press, Oxford
Simon TJ, Hespos SJ, Rochat P (1995) Do infants understand arithmetic? A replication of Wynn (1992). Cogn Dev 10:253–269
Sobel ME (1982) Asymptotic confidence intervals for indirect effects in structural equation models. Sociol Methodol 13:290–312
Thomas RK (1992) Primates’ conceptual use of number: ecological perspectives and psychological processes. In: Nishida T, McGrew WC, Marler P, Pickfbrd M, de Waal FBM (eds) Topics in primatology, vol. 1: human origins. Tokyo University Press, Tokyo, pp 305–314
Tomasello M, Call J (1997) Primate cognition. Oxford University Press, Oxford
Uller C (2008) Developmental and evolutionary considerations on numerical cognition: a review. J Evol Psychol 6(4):237–253
Uller C, Lewis J (2009) Horses (Equus caballus) select the greater of two quantities in small numerical contrasts. Anim Cogn 12(5):733–738
Uller C, Carey S, Huntley-Fenner G, Klatt L (1999) What representations might underlie infant numerical knowledge. Cogn Dev 14(1):1–36
Ward C, Smuts B (2007) Quantity-based judgments in the domestic dog (Canis lupus familiaris). Anim Cogn 10:71–80
Watts D, Mitani J (2002) Hunting behaviour of chimpanzees at Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda. Int J Primatol 23:1–28
Wiese H (2003) Iconic and non-iconic stages in number development: the role of language. Trends Cogn Sci 7:385–390
Wilson M, Hauser M, Wrangham R (2007) Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) modify grouping and vocal behaviour in response to location-specific risk. Behaviour 144:1621–1653
Xu F, Carey S (1996) Infants’ metaphysics: the case of numerical identity. Cogn Psychol 30:111–153
Acknowledgments
We are grateful to the directors of the L’Oceanografic aquarium of Valencia and Zoo Aquarium of Madrid for allowing us to conduct this research. Special thanks go to Miguel Bueno Brinkman and Manuel Castellote for their help and support and to José Antonio Esteban for help with video recordings and for providing very helpful ideas that contributed to the design of this study. We appreciate the work of the animal keepers from the two aquariums. From L’Oceanografic aquarium of Valencia, head coach Victor Sánchez, for sea lions training and management and the trainers team; Jerome Coello, Javier Escudero, Ana Coba, Luciana Suárez and Sandra Salido. Zoo Aquarium of Madrid head coach Carlos de las Parras for sea lions training and management and the trainers team; Arancha Sánchez, Cacha Martínez, Berta Romeral, Pablo Juárez, Pablo Roy, Antonio Martínez, Irene Ayllon and Craig Allum. In addition, we thank Luis Franco for his enormous help in the design and manufacturing of the apparatus used in this study. Thanks to Rosario Martínez Arias and Ma José Hernández-Lloreda for their comments and statistical advice. Special thanks go to Titaví for her artwork. Research reported in this study was partly supported by FPI studentship to José Francisco Zamorano Abramson, and project grants CCG08-UCM/SAL-4007 (Universidad Complutense de Madrid y Comunidad de Madrid) to M. V. Hernández-Lloreda, and UCM-BSCH GR58/08 (Universidad Complutense de Madrid y Banco Santander Central Hispano) and D/018712/08 AECID (Agencia Española de Cooperación Internacional y Desarrollo), Spain to F. Colmenares.
Conflict of interest
The authors declared that they had no conflicts of interests with respect to their authorship and/or the publication of this article.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding authors
Additional information
The experiments comply with the current laws of Spain.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Abramson, J.Z., Hernández-Lloreda, V., Call, J. et al. Relative quantity judgments in South American sea lions (Otaria flavescens). Anim Cogn 14, 695–706 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-011-0404-7
Received:
Revised:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-011-0404-7