Abstract
A series of place learning experiments was carried out in young chicks (Gallus gallus) in order to investigate how the geometry of a landmark array and that of a walled enclosure compete when disoriented animals could rely on both of them to re-orient towards the centre of the enclosure. A square-shaped array (four wooden sticks) was placed in the middle of a square-shaped enclosure, the two structures being concentric. Chicks were trained to ground-scratch to search for food hidden in the centre of the enclosure (and the array). To check for effects of array degradation, one, two, three or all landmarks were removed during test trials. Chicks concentrated their searching activity in the central area of the enclosure, but their accuracy was inversely contingent on the number of landmarks removed; moreover, the landmarks still present within the enclosure appeared to influence the shape of the searching patterns. The reduction in the number of landmarks affected the searching strategy of chicks, suggesting that they had focussed mainly on local cues when landmarks were present within the enclosure. When all the landmarks were removed, chicks searched over a larger area, suggesting an absolute encoding of distances from the local cues and less reliance on the relationships provided by the geometry of the enclosure. Under conditions of monocular vision, chicks tended to rely on different strategies to localize the centre on the basis of the eye (and thus the hemisphere) in use, the left hemisphere attending to details of the environment and the right hemisphere attending to the global shape.
Similar content being viewed by others
References
Andrew RJ (1991) The nature of behavioural lateralization in the chick. In: Andrew RJ (ed) Neural and behavioural plasticity: the use of the chick as a model. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 536–554
Benhamou S, Poucet B (1998) Landmark use by navigating rats (Rattus norvegicus): contrasting geometric and featural information. J Comp Psychol 112:317–322
Bennett ATD (1993) Spatial memory in a food-storing corvid: I. Near tall landmarks are primarily used. J Comp Physiol A 173:193–207
Cheng K (1986) A purely geometric module in the rat's spatial representation. Cognition 23:149–178
Cheng K (2005) Reflections on geometry and navigation. Connect Sci 17:5–21
Cheng K, Newcombe NS (2005) Is there a geometric module for spatial orientation? Squaring theory and evidence. Psychon Bull Rev 12:1–23
Collett TS, Cartwright BA, Smith BA (1986) Landmark learning and visuo-spatial memories in gerbils. J Comp Physiol A 158:835–851
Deng C, Rogers LJ (1998a) Organisation of the tectorotundal and SP/IPS-rotundal projections in the chick. J Comp Neurol 394:171–185
Della Chiesa A, Speranza M, Tommasi L, Vallortigara G (2006) Spatial cognition based on geometry and landmarks in the domestic chick (Gallus gallus). Behav Brain Res, in press
Deng C, Rogers LJ (1998b) Bilaterally projecting neurons in the two visual pathways of chicks. Brain Res 794:281–290
Gallistel CR (1990) The organization of learning. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA
Goodyear AJ, Kamil AC (2004) Clark's nutcrackers (Nucifraga columbiana) and the effects of goal-landmark distance on overshadowing. J Comp Psychol 118:258–264
Kamil AC, Cheng K (2001) Way-finding and landmarks: the multiple-bearings hypothesis. J Exp Biol 204:103–113
Kamil AC, Jones JE (1997) Clark's nutcrackers learn geometric relationships among landmarks. Nature 390:276–79
Kamil AC, Jones JE (2000) Geometric rule learning by Clark's nutcrackers (Nucifraga columbiana). J Exp Psychol: Anim Behav Process 26:439–453
Kamil AC, Goodyear AJ, Cheng K (2001) The use of landmarks by Clark's nutcrackers: first tests of a new model. J Navigat 54:429–435
Jones JE, Antoniadis E, Shettleworth SJ, Kamil AC (2002) A comparative study of geometric rule learning by nutcrackers (Nucifraga columbiana), pigeons (Columba livia), and jackdaws (Corvus monedula). J Comp Psychol 116:350–356
Rogers LJ (1995) The development of brain and behaviour in the chicken. CAB International, Wallingford, UK
Sovrano VA, Vallortigara G (2006) Dissecting the geometric module: a sense-linkage for metric and landmark information in animals’ spatial reorientation. Psych Sci 17:616--621
Sovrano VA, Bisazza A, Vallortigara G (2002) Modularity and spatial reorientation in a simple mind: encoding of geometric and nongeometric properties of a spatial environment by fish. Cogn 85:B51–B59
Sovrano VA, Bisazza A, Vallortigara G (2003) Modularity as a fish views it: conjoining geometric and nongeometric information for spatial reorientation. J Exp Psychol: Anim Behav Process 29:199–210
Sovrano VA, Bisazza A, Vallortigara G (2005) Animals’ use of landmarks and metric information to reorient: effects of the size of the experimental space. Cogn 97:121–133
Sovrano VA, Bisazza A, Vallortigara G (2006) How fish do geometry in large and small spaces. Anim Cogn, DOI 10.1007/s10071-006-0029-4
Spetch ML, Cheng K, MacDonald SE, Linkenhoker BA, Kelly DM, Doerkson SR (1997) Use of landmark configuration in pigeons and humans: II. Generality across search tasks. J Comp Psychol 111:114–124
Spetch ML, Rust TB, Kamil AC, Jones JE (2003) Searching by rules: pigeons’ (Columba livia) landmark-based search according to constant bearing or constant distance. J Comp Psychol 117:123–132
Tommasi L, Gagliardo A, Andrew RJ, Vallortigara G (2003) Separate processing mechanisms for encoding geometric and landmark information in the avian hippocampus. Eur J Neurosci 17:1695–1702
Tommasi L, Vallortigara G (2000) Searching for the center: spatial cognition in the domestic chick (Gallus gallus). J Exp Psychol: Anim Behav Process 26:477–486
Tommasi L, Vallortigara (2001) Encoding of geometric and landmark information in the left and right hemispheres of the Avian brain. Behav Neurosci 115:602–613
Tommasi L, Vallortigara (2004) Hemispheric processing of landmark and geometric information in male and female domestic chicks (Gallus gallus). Behav Brain Res 155:85–96
Tommasi L, Vallortigara G, Zanforlin M (1997) Young chickens learn to localize the centre of a spatial environment. J Comp Physiol A 180:567–572
Vallortigara G (2000) Comparative neuropsychology of the dual brain: a stroll through left and right animals’ perceptual worlds. Brain Lang 73:189–219
Vallortigara G (2004) Visual cognition and representation in birds and primates. In: Rogers LJ, Kaplan G (eds) Vertebrate comparative cognition: are primates superior to non-primates? Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers, pp 57–94
Vallortigara G (2006) The cognitive chicken: visual and spatial cognition in a non-mammalian brain. In: Wasserman EA, Zentall TR (eds) Comparative cognition: experimental explorations of animal intelligence. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK, pp 41–58
Vallortigara G, Sovrano VA (2002) Conjoining information from different modules: a comparative perspective. Behav Brain Sci 25:701–702
Vallortigara G, Feruglio M, Sovrano VA (2005) Reorientation by geometric and landmark information in environments of different size. Dev Sci 8:393–401
Vallortigara G, Zanforlin M, Pasti G (1990) Geometric modules in animal's spatial representation: a test with chicks. J Comp Psychol 104:248–254
Vallortigara G, Pagni P, Sovrano VA (2004) Separate geometric and non-geometric modules for spatial reorientation: evidence from a lopsided animal brain. J Cogn Neurosci 16:390–400
Watanabe S, Huber L (2006) Animal logics: decisions in the absence of human language. Anim Cogn, DOI 10.1007/s10071-006-0043-6
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Additional information
This contribution is part of the special issue “Animal Logics” (Watanabe and Huber 2006).
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Chiesa, A., Pecchia, T., Tommasi, L. et al. Multiple landmarks, the encoding of environmental geometry and the spatial logics of a dual brain. Anim Cogn 9, 281–293 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-006-0050-7
Received:
Revised:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-006-0050-7