Abstract
We examined the ability of domestic dogs to choose the larger versus smaller quantity of food in two experiments. In experiment 1, we investigated the ability of 29 dogs (results from 18 dogs were used in the data analysis) to discriminate between two quantities of food presented in eight different combinations. Choices were simultaneously presented and visually available at the time of choice. Overall, subjects chose the larger quantity more often than the smaller quantity, but they found numerically close comparisons more difficult. In experiment 2, we tested two dogs from experiment 1 under three conditions. In condition 1, we used similar methods from experiment 1 and tested the dogs multiple times on the eight combinations from experiment 1 plus one additional combination. In conditions 2 and 3, the food was visually unavailable to the subjects at the time of choice, but in condition 2, food choices were viewed simultaneously before being made visually unavailable, and in condition 3, they were viewed successively. In these last two conditions, and especially in condition 3, the dogs had to keep track of quantities mentally in order to choose optimally. Subjects still chose the larger quantity more often than the smaller quantity when the food was not simultaneously visible at the time of choice. Olfactory cues and inadvertent cuing by the experimenter were excluded as mechanisms for choosing larger quantities. The results suggest that, like apes tested on similar tasks, some dogs can form internal representations and make mental comparisons of quantity.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.



Similar content being viewed by others
References
American Kennel Club (1992) The complete dog book, 18th edn. Howell Book House, New York
Bart H, La Mont K, Kipton J, Dehaene S, Kanwisher N, Spelke E (2006) Non-symbolic arithmetic in adults and young children. Cognition 98:199–222
Beach FA, Beuhler MG, Dunbar I (1983) Development of attraction to estrous females in male dogs. Physiol Behav 31:293–297
Beran MJ (2001) Summation and numerousness judgments of sequentially presented sets of items by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J Comp Psychol 115:181–191
Beran MJ (2004) Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) respond to nonvisible sets after one-by-one addition and removal of items. J Comp Psychol 118:25–36
Boysen ST, Berntson GG (1995) Responses to quantity: perceptual versus cognitive mechanisms in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 21:82–86
Brodin A (2005) Mechanisms of cache retrieval in long-term hoarding birds. J Ethol 23:77–83
Call J (2000) Estimating and operating on discrete quantities in orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus). J Comp Psychol 114:136–147
Cordes S, Gelman R, Gallistel CR, Whalen J (2001) Variability signatures distinguish verbal from non-verbal counting for both large and small number. Psychon Bull Rev 8:698–707
Davis H (1984) Determination of the number three by a raccoon (Procyon lotor). Anim Learn Behav 12:409–413
Dehaene S (1997) The number sense. Oxford University Press, Oxford
Dehaene S, Dehaene-Lambertz G, Cohen L (1998) Abstract representations of number in the animal and human brain. Trends Neurosci 21:355–361
Diggle PJ, Liang K, Zeger SL (1994) Analysis of longitudinal data. Oxford University Press, Oxford
Dooley GB, Gill T (1977) Acquisition and use of mathematical skills by a linguistic chimpanzee. In: Rumbaugh DM (ed) Language learning by a chimpanzee. Academic Press, New York, pp 247–260
Feigenson L, Carey S, Hauser M (2002) The representations underlying infants’ choice of more: object files versus analog magnitudes. Psychol Sci 13:150–156
Fiset S, Beaulieu C, Landry F (2003) Duration of dogs’ (Canis familiaris) working memory in search for disappearing objects. Anim Cogn 6:1–10
Fox MW (1971) Behaviour of wolves, dogs and related canids. Harper and Row, NY
Gallistel CR, Gelman R (2000) Non-verbal numerical cognition: from reals to integers. Trends Cogn Sci 4:59–65
Gould-Beierle K (2000) A comparison of four corvid species in a working and reference memory task using a radial maze. J Comp Psychol 114:347–356
Hardin JW, Hilbe JM (2002) Generalized estimating equations. Chapman & Hall, Boca Raton, FL
Hare B, Brown M, Williamson C, Tomasello M (2002) The domestication of social cognition in dogs. Science 298:1634–1636
Harrington FH, Mech LB (1979) Wolf howling and its role in territory maintenance. Behaviour 68:207–249
Hauser MD (2003) Primate cognition. In: Gallagher M, Nelson RJ (eds) Comprehensive handbook of psychology: biological psychology. Wiley, New York, pp 561–594
Hauser MD, MacNeilage P, Hauser LB (2000) Spontaneous number representation in semi-free ranging rhesus monkeys. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 267:829–833
Hepper PG (1986) Sibling recognition in the domestic dog. Anim Behav 34:288–289
Huntley-Fenner G, Cannon E (2000) Preschoolers’ magnitude comparisons are mediated by a preverbal analog mechanism. Psychol Sci 11:147–152
Jordan KE, Brannon EM, Logothetis NK, Ghazanfar AA (2005) Monkeys match the number of voices they hear to the number of faces they see. Curr Biol 15:1034–1038
Jordan KE, Brannon EM (2006a) Weber's Law influences numerical representations in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Anim Cogn 9:159–172
Jordan KE, Brannon EM (2006b) The multisensory representation of number in infancy. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 103:3486–3489
Kilian A, Yaman S, von Fersen L, Gunturkun O (2003) A bottlenose dolphin discriminates visual stimuli differing in numerosity. Learn Behav 31:133–142
Kitchen DM (2004) Alpha male black howler monkey responses to loud calls: effect of numeric odds, male companion behaviour and reproductive investment. Anim Behav 67:125–139
Kowalska DM (1995) Effects of hippocampal lesions on spatial delayed responses in dog. Hippocampus 5:363–370
Kowalska DM (2000) Cognitive functions of the temporal lobe in the dog: a review. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 24:855–880
Lewis KP, Jaffe S, Brannon EB (2005) Analog number representations in mongoose lemurs (Eulemur mongoz): evidence from a search task. Anim Cogn 8:247–252
Lyon BE (2003) Egg recognition and counting reduce costs of avian conspecific brood parasitism. Nature 422:495–499
Macdonald DW, Carr GM (1995) Variation in dog society: between resource dispersion and social flux. In: Serpell J (ed) The domestic dog: its evolution, behaviour, and interactions with people. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 199–216
McCartney W (1968) Olfaction and odours. Springer-Verlag, Berlin
McComb K, Packer C, Pusey A (1994) Roaring and numerical assessment in contests between groups of female lions, Panthera leo. Anim Behav 47:379–387
Meck WH, Church RM (1983) A mode control model of counting and timing processes. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 9:320–334
Meier TJ, Burch JW, Mech LD, Adams LG (1995) Pack structure, dynamics and genetic relatedness among wolf packs in a naturally regulated population. In: Carbyn LN, Fritts SH, Seip DSR (eds) Ecology and conservation of wolves in a changing world. Canadian Circumpolar Institute, Edmonton, Alberta, pp 293–302
Moyer RS, Landauer TK (1967) Time required for judgments of numerical inequality. Nature 215:1519–1520
Passe DH, Walker JC (1985) Odor psychophysics in vertebrates. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 9:431–467
Pepperberg IM (1987) Evidence for conceptual quantitative abilities in the African grey parrot: labeling of cardinal sets. Ethology 75:37–61
Rumbaugh DM, Savage-Rumbaugh S, Hegel MT (1987) Summation in the chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 13:107–115
Shumaker RW, Palkovich AM, Beck BB, Guagnano GA, Morowitz H (2001) Spontaneous use of magnitude discrimination and ordination by the orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus). J Comp Psychol 115:385–391
Siegler RS, Opfer JE (2003) The development of numerical estimation: evidence for multiple representations of numerical quantity. Psychol Sci 14:237–243
Smith DW, Ferguson G (2005) Decade of the wolf: returning the wild to Yellowstone. The Lyons Press, Guilford, CT
Stephens D, Krebs JR (1986). Foraging theory. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ
Vilà C, Maldonado JE, Wayne RK (1999) Phylogenetic relationships, evolution, and genetic diversity of the domestic dog. J Hered 90:71–77
West RE, Young RJ (2002) Do domestic dogs show any evidence of being able to count? Anim Cogn 5:183–186
Wilson ML, Hauser MD, Wrangham RW (2001) Does participation in intergroup conflict depend on numerical assessment, range location, or rank for wild chimpanzees? Anim Behav 61:1203–1216
Acknowledgments
This study was supported by the University of Michigan's Rackham Graduate School. We wish to thank all of the owners and their dogs for their participation in the study. Thanks to Kathy Welch at the University of Michigan for her statistical input. These experiments comply with the current laws of the United States and the standards established by the University of Michigan's Committee on Use and Care of Animals (project approval no. 8792).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Ward, C., Smuts, B.B. Quantity-based judgments in the domestic dog (Canis lupus familiaris). Anim Cogn 10, 71–80 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-006-0042-7
Received:
Revised:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-006-0042-7