Abstract
This paper examines the contribution of stimulus processing to animal logics. In the classic functionalist S-O-R view of learning (and cognition), stimuli provide the raw material to which the organism applies its cognitive processes–its logic, which may be taxon-specific. Stimuli may contribute to the logic of the organism's response, and may do so in taxon-specific ways. Firstly, any non-trivial stimulus has an internal organization that may constrain or bias the way that the organism addresses it; since stimuli can only be defined relative to the organism's perceptual apparatus, and this apparatus is taxon-specific, such constraints or biases will often be taxon-specific. Secondly, the representation of a stimulus that the perceptual system builds, and the analysis it makes of this representation, may provide a model for the synthesis and analysis done at a more cognitive level. Such a model is plausible for evolutionary reasons: perceptual analysis was probably perfected before cognitive analysis in the evolutionary history of the vertebrates. Like stimulus-driven analysis, such perceptually modelled cognition may be taxon-specific because of the taxon-specificity of the perceptual apparatus. However, it may also be the case that different taxa are able to free themselves from the stimulus logic, and therefore apply a more abstract logic, to different extents. This thesis is defended with reference to two examples of cases where animals' cognitive logic seems to be isomorphic with perceptual logic, specifically in the case of pigeons' attention to global and local information in visual stimuli, and dogs' failure to comprehend means-end relationships in string-pulling tasks.
Similar content being viewed by others
References
Adams DK (1929) Experimental studies of adaptive behavior in cats. Comp Psychol Monog 6(1):1–128
Aust U, Huber L (2001) The role of item- and category-specific information in the discrimination of people versus nonpeople images by pigeons. Anim Learn Behav 29:107–119
Birch HG (1945) The relation of previous experience to insightful problem solving. J Comp Psychol 38:367–383
Bitterman ME (1975) The comparative analysis of learning. Science 188:699–709
Blasi A, Hoeffel EC (1974) Adolescence and formal operations. Hum Dev 17:344–363
Cavoto KK, Cook RG (2001) Cognitive precedence for local information in hierarchical stimulus processing by pigeons. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Proc 27:3–16
Cerella J (1980) The pigeon's analysis of pictures. Pattern Recogn 12:1–6 (DOI: 10.1016/0031-3203(80)90048-5)
Cook RG, Wright AA, Kendrick DF (1990) Visual categorization in pigeons. In: Commons ML, Herrnstein RJ, Kosslyn SM, Mumford DB (eds) Quantitative analyses of behavior: behavioral approaches to pattern recognition and concept formation, vol 8. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp 187–214
Danner FW, Day MC (1977) Eliciting formal operations. Child Dev 48:1600–1606
Darwin C (1871) The descent of man and selection in relation to sex. John Murray, London
Darwin C (1975) The origin of species. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA
Delius JD, Hollard VD (1995) Orientation invariant pattern-recognition by pigeons (Columba livia) and humans (Homo sapiens). J Comp Psychol 109:278–290
Deruelle C, Fagot J (1998) Visual search for global/local stimulus features in humans and baboons. Psychonom Bull Rev 5:476–481
Doré FY, Dumas C (1987) Psychology of animal cognition: piagetian studies. Psychol Bull 102:219–233
Ducker G, Rensch B (1977) Solution of patterned string problems by birds. Behaviour 62:164–173
Eddy TJ, Gallup GG, Povinelli DJ (1993) Attribution of cognitive states to animals: anthropomorphism in comparative perspective. J Soc Issues 49:87–101
Emery NJ (2006) Cognitive ornithology: the evolution of avian intelligence. Phil Trans R Soc Ser B 361:23–43
Fagot J, Deruelle C (1997) Processing of global and local visual information and hemispheric specialization in humans (Homo sapiens) and baboons (Papio papio). J Exp Psychol: Hum Percept Perform 23:429–442
Fagot J, Tomonaga M (1999) Global and local processing in humans (homo sapiens) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): use of a visual search task with compound stimuli. J Comp Psychol 113:3–12
Fischel W (1933) The behavior of dogs with double goal setting and two possible actions/Das Verhalten von Hunden bei doppelter Zielsetzung und doppelter Handlungsmoeglichkeit. Z Vergl Physiol 19:170–182
Fujita K, Ushitani T (2005) Better living by not completing: a wonderful peculiarity of pigeon vision? Behav Proc 69:59–66
Funk MS (2002) Problem solving skills in young yellow-crowned parakeets (Cyanoramphus auriceps). Anim Cogn 5:167–176 (DOI: 10.1007/s10071-002-0149-4)
Ghosh N, Lea SEG, Noury M (2004) Transfer to intermediate forms following concept discrimination by pigeons: Chimeras and morphs. J Exp Anal Behav 82:125–141
Goto K (2004) Processing of global and local features in pictures and movies by pigeons. Unpublished PhD dissertation, University of Exeter, Exeter, UK
Goto K, Lea SEG (2003) Discrimination of direction of movements in pigeons following previous experience of motion/static discrimination. J Exp Anal Behav 80:29–42
Goto K, Wills AJ, Lea SEG (2004) Global-feature classification can be acquired more rapidly than local-feature classification in both humans and pigeons. Anim Cogn 7:109–113
Hamm J, Matheson WR, Honig WK (1997) Mental rotation in pigeons (Columba livia)? J Comp Psychol 111:76–81
Hauser MD, Kralik J, Botto-Mahan C (1999) Problem solving and functional design features: experiments on cotton-top tamarins, Saguinus oedipus oedipus. Anim Behav 57:565–582
Hauser MD, Santos LR, Spaepen GM, Pearson HE (2002) Problem solving, inhibition and domain-specific experience: experiments on cottontop tamarins, Saguinus oedipus. Anim Behav 64:387–396
Heinrich B (1995) An experimental investigation of insight in common ravens (Corvus corax). Auk 112:994–1003
Hollard VD, Delius JD (1982) Rotational invariance in visual-pattern recognition by pigeons and humans. Science 218:804–806
Hopkins WD (1997) Hemispheric specialization for local and global processing of hierarchical visual stimuli in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Neuropsychologia 35:343–348
Hopkins WD, Washburn DA (2002) Matching visual stimuli on the basis of global and local features by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Anim Cogn 5:27–31
Hubel DH, Wiesel TN (1959) Receptive fields of single neurones in the cat's striate cortex. J Physiol 148:574–591
Huber L, Gajdon GK (2006) Technical intelligence in animals: the kea model. Anim Cogn DOI 10.1007/s10071-006-0033-8
Kamil AC (1988) A synthetic approach to the study of animal intelligence. In: Leger DW (ed) Nebraska symposium on motivation: comparative perspectives in modern psychology, vol 35. University of Nebraska Press, Lincoln, NE, pp 258–308
Kamil AC (1998) On the proper definition of cognitive ethology. In: Balda RP, Pepperberg IM, Kamil AC (eds) Animal cognition in nature: the convergence of psychology and biology in laboratory and field. Academic Press, San Diego, CA, pp 1–28
Kelly DM, Cook RG (2003) Differential effects of visual context on pattern discrimination by pigeons (Columba livia) and humans (Homo sapiens). J Comp Psychol 117:200–208
Köhler W (1927) The mentality of apes, 2nd edn. Routledge and Kegan Paul, London
Köhler W (1969) The task of Gestalt psychology. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ
Kirkpatrick K (2001) Object recognition. In: Cook RG (ed) Avian visual cognition. Available via www.pigeon.psy.tufts.edu/avc/kirkpatrick/
Lehar S (2003) Gestalt isomorphism and the primacy of subjective conscious experience: a Gestalt Bubble model. Behav Brain Sci 26:375–444
Lettvin JY, Maturana HR, McCulloch WS, Pitts WH (1959) What the frog's eye tells the frog's brain. Proc IRE 47:1940–1951
Matsukawa A, Inoue S, Jitsumori M (2004) Pigeon's recognition of cartoons: effects of fragmentation, scrambling, and deletion of elements. Behav Proc 65:25–34 (DOI: 10.1016/S0376-6357(03)00147-5)
Mauck B, Dehnhardt G (1997) Mental rotation in a California sea lion (Zalophus californianus). J Exp Biol 200:1309–1316
Munakata Y, McClelland JL, Johnson MH, Siegler RS (1997) Rethinking infant knowledge: towards an adaptive process account of successes and failures in object permanence tasks. Poychol Rev 104:686–713
Nakajima S, Arimitsu K, Lattal KM (2002) Estimation of animal intelligence by university students in Japan and the United States. Anthrozoös 15:194–205
Navon D (1977) Forest before trees: the precedence of global features in visual perception. Cogn Psychol 9:353–383
Osthaus B, Lea SEG, Slater AM (2005) Dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) fail to show understanding of means-end connections in a string-pulling task. Anim Cogn 8:37–47 (DOI: 10.1007/s10071-004-0230-2)
Pack A, Herman LM, Hoffmann-Kuhnt M, Branstetter BK (2002) The object behind the echo: dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) perceive object shape globally through echolocation. Behav Proc 58:1–26
Pepperberg IM (2004) "Insightful" string-pulling in Grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus) is affected by vocal competence. Anim Cogn 7:263–266 (DOI: 10.1007/s10071-004-0218-y)
Piaget J (1972) Intellectual evolution from adolescence to adulthood. Hum Dev 15:1–12
Piaget J, Inhelder B (1969) The psychology of the child. Routledge, London
Scott JP, Fuller JL (1965) Genetics and the social behavior of the dog. University of Chicago Press, Chicago
Sekuler AB, Lee JAJ, Shettleworth SJ (1996) Pigeons do not complete partly occluded figures. Perception 25:1109–1120
Shepherd WT (1915) Tests on adaptive intelligence in dogs and cats, as compared with adaptive intelligence in Rhesus monkeys. Am J Psychol 26:211–216
Shettleworth SJ (1993) Where is the comparison in comparative cognition? Psychol Sci 4:179–184
Shettleworth SJ (1998) Cognition, evolution and behavior. Oxford University Press, New York
Skinner BF (1969) Contingencies of reinforcement: a theoretical analysis. Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York
Spencer J, Quinn PC, Johnson MH, Karmiloff-Smith A (1997) Heads you win, tails you lose: evidence for young infants categorizing mammals by head and facial attributes. Early Dev Parent 6:113–126
Spinozzi G, Poti P (1993) Piagetian stage 5 in 2 infant chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)–the development of permanence of objects and the spatialisation of causality. Int J Primatol 14:905–917
Spinozzi G, DeLillo C, Truppa V (2003) Global and local processing of hierarchical visual stimuli in tufted Capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). J Comp Psychol 117:15–23
Thorndike EL (1911) Animal intelligence: experimental studies. Macmillan, New York
Van Hamme LJ, Wasserman EA, Biederman I (1992) Discrimination of contour-deleted images by pigeons. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Proc 18:387–399
Vauclair J, Fagot J, Hopkins WD (1993) Rotation of mental images in baboons when the visual input is directed to the left cerebral hemisphere. Psychol Sci 4:99–103
Vince MA (1961) "String-pulling" in birds: III. The successful response in greenfinches and canaries. Behaviour 17:103–129
Wason PC, Shapiro D (1971) Natural and contrived experience in a reasoning task. Q J Exp Psychol 23:63–71
Wasserman EA (1993) Comparative cognition: beginning the second century of the study of animal intelligence. Psychol Bull 113:211–228
Wasserman EA, Kirkpatrick-Steger K, Van Hamme LJ, Biederman I (1993) Pigeons are sensitive to the spatial organization of complex visual stimuli. Psychol Sci 4:336–341
Watanabe S (2001a) Discrimination of cartoons and photographs in pigeons: effects of scrambling of elements. Behav Proc 53:3–9
Watanabe S (2001b) Van Gogh, Chagall and pigeons: picture discrimination in pigeons and humans. Anim Cogn 4:147–151
Watanabe S, Huber L (2006). Animal logics: decisions in the absence of human language. Anim Cogn DOI 10.1007/s10071-006-0043-6
Willatts P (1999) Development of means-end behavior in young infants: pulling a support to retrieve a distant object. Dev Psychol 35:651–667
Woodworth RS (1929) Psychology, 2nd edn. Holt, New York
Wright AA (1972) The influence of ultraviolet radiation on the pigeon's color discrimination. J Exp Anal Behav 17:325–337
Acknowledgements
This paper is based on one delivered at the symposium on animal logics held at the Konrad Lorenz Institute for Evolution and Cognition Research, Altenberg, Austria, in November 2004. The ideas presented here owe much to discussion with colleagues, notably Andy Wills, Alan Slater and Lucia Jacobs. Preparation of the manuscript was supported by European Communities Framework 6 (NEST) project 516542, “From Associations to Rules”, and US National Institute of Mental Health grant MH068426. Kazuhiro Goto is now at Keio University.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Additional information
This contribution is part of the special issue “Animal Logics” (Watanabe and Huber 2006).
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Lea, S., Goto, K., Osthaus, B. et al. The logic of the stimulus. Anim Cogn 9, 247–256 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-006-0038-3
Received:
Revised:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-006-0038-3