Skip to main content
Log in

Video analysis of endoscopic cutting task performed by one versus two operators

  • Original article
  • Published:
Surgical Endoscopy And Other Interventional Techniques Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Background

In the performance of complex laparoscopic tasks, one question is whether the task should be distributed between two operators or accomplished bimanually by one operator. The authors hypothesized that superior task performance results when two operators work collaboratively in a dyad team as opposed to one operator performing the task bimanually. Furthermore, in a visually misaligned condition, the performance of a team will be more robust than that of a single operator working alone.

Methods

The suture-cutting task was performed by 24 right-handed subjects in a mock surgical setup using a laparoscopic grasper and a pair of laparoscopic scissors. The cutting task was performed by 8 subjects bimanually (using both limbs) and 16 paired subjects unimanually (using their preferred limbs). The image of the work plane was displayed either vertically or superimposed over the work plane. In half of the conditions, the camera was rotated 45°, causing misalignment between the actual and displayed work planes. Movements were videotaped. Important movement events were identified and used to subdivide the task into subtasks. Durations of the subtasks and attempts for grasping and cutting were analyzed using a mixed-design multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA).

Results

For a number of subtasks, the dyad group showed shorter durations than the bimanual group. The 45° rotation of the camera degraded both bimanual and dyad performance, resulting in prolonged movement times for all subtasks. The learning process was facilitated by the superimposed display in that grasper and scissor reaching times improved over trials, as compared with the vertical display.

Conclusion

The results indicate the superior role of team collaboration, as compared with the single operator, in a complex remote manipulation such as a laparoscopic cutting task. This enhanced task performance is achieved because of the larger capacity for information processing. These results may have some relevance for optimizing performance of endoscopic surgery.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Figure 1
Figure 2
Figure 3
Fig.4. As

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Bholat OS, Haluck RS, Murray WB, Gorman PJ, Kurmmel TM (1999) Tactile feedback is present during minimally invasive surgery. J Am Coll Surg, 189: 349–355

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Bowers CA, Braun CC, Morgan BB (1997) Team workload: its meaning and measurement. In: Brannick MT, Salas E, Prince C (eds). Team performance assessment and measurement: theory, methods, and applications. Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ pp 85–108

    Google Scholar 

  3. Breedveld P, Wentink M (2001) Eye–hand coordination in laparoscopy: an overview of experiments and supporting aids. Min Invas Ther Allied Technol 10: 155–162

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Cao CGL, MacKenzie CL, Ibbotson JA, Turner LJ, Blair NP, Nagy AG (1999) Hierarchical decomposition of laparoscopic procedures. In: Westwood JD, Hoffman HM, Robb RA, Stredney D (eds.) Medicine meets virtual reality 7: the convergence of physical and informational technologies: options for a new era in healthcare. IOS Press, Amsterdam, pp 83–89

    Google Scholar 

  5. Carter AJ, West MA (1999) Sharing the burden: teamwork in health care settings. In: Firth-Cozens J, Payne RL (eds) Stress in health professional. John Wiley, Chichester, England pp 191–202

    Google Scholar 

  6. Cresswell AB, Macmillan AIM, Hanna GB, Cuschieri A (1999) Methods for improving performance under reverse alignment conditions during endoscopic surgery. Surg Endosc 13: 591–594

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Cuschieri A, (1995) Visual displays and visual perception in minimal access surgery. Semin Laparosc Surg 2: 209–214

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Gallagher AC, McClure N, McGuigan J (1998) An ergonomic analysis of the “fulcrum effect” in endoscopic skill acquisition. Endoscopy 30: 617–620

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Guiard Y, (1987) Asymmetric division of labor in human skilled bimanual action: the kinematic chain as a model. J Mot Behav 19: 486–517

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Hanna GB, Shimi S, Cuschieri A (1997). Influence of direction of view, target-to-endoscope distance and manipulation angle on endoscopic knot tying. Br J Surg 84: 1406–1464

    Google Scholar 

  11. Janmohamed Z, Zheng B, MacKenzie CL (2003) Aiming with the hand touching the target: the role of proprioceptive information between hands. Proceedings of the 34th Association of Canadian Ergonomists Annual Conference, October 15, 2003, London, Ontario

    Google Scholar 

  12. Kelso JA, Southard DL, Goodman D (1979) On the coordination of two-handed movements. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 5: 229 –238

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Lederman SJ, Klatzky RL (1987) Hand movement: a window into haptic object recognition. Cognit Psychol. 19: 342–368

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. MacKenzie C, Iberall T (1994) The grasping hand. North-Holland, Amsterdam

  15. Mandryk RL, MacKenzie CL (1999) Superimposing display space on workspace in the context of endoscopic surgery. CHI Companion, CHI ′99. ACM Press, New York pp. 284–285

    Google Scholar 

  16. Morgan BB Jr, Salas E, Glickman AS (1994) An analysis of team evolution and maturation. J Gen Psychol, 120: 277–291

    Google Scholar 

  17. Norman DA, Bobrow DG (1975). On data-limited and resource-limited processes. Cognit Psychol 7: 44–64

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Pashler H (1994) Dual-task interference in simple task: data and theory. Psychol Bull 116: 220–224

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Tendick F, Jennings RW, Tharp G, Stark L (1993). Sensing and manipulation problems in endoscopic surgery: experiment, analysis, and observation. Presence 2: 66–81

    Google Scholar 

  20. Wiesendanger M, Kanzennikov O, Perrig S, Kaluzny P (1996) Two hands, one action. In: Wing AM, Haggard P, Flanagan JR (eds) Hand and brain. Academic Press, San Diago CA pp 283–300

    Google Scholar 

  21. Zheng B, Janmohamed Z, MacKenzie CL (2003) Reaction times and the decision-making process in endoscopic surgery. Surg Endosc 17: 1475–1480

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by the Michael Smith Foundation for Health Research of British Columbia, Canada, through a postgraduate scholarship to B. Zheng, and by Canada’s NCE Institute for Robotics and Intelligent Systems (IRIS) grants to C. L. MacKenzie at Simon Fraser University.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to C. L. MacKenzie.

Appendix

Appendix

Temporal data were recorded from the VCR counterclock, which was synchronized with the video recordings. The counter was based on 60 min, 60 s, and the number of frames (30 frame/s time intervals), with the counter increasing as the time duration increased. Thus, once the counter read 00:00:29, the next frame increased the counter to read 1 s, indicated by 00:01:00. When counter read 00:59:29, the next frame increased the counter to 01:00:00. In contrast, when temporal data are inputted to Microsoft EXCEL, in which data are sorted and analyzed, EXCEL requires data to be in the format of 60:60:60 (conventional time format) for the software to “recognize” it. Therefore, when inputting VCR counterclock data into EXCEL, we multiplied the last two digits by 2 to make it a ratio of 60 rather than 30. Time durations between defined events were then calculated by subtracting the respective time codes and converted them to seconds.

The conversion steps are as follows:

VCR counterclock = 00:31:27

Total time duration = 00:31:54

= 31 seconds + 54/60 seconds

= 31.9 seconds

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Zheng, B., Verjee, F., Lomax, A. et al. Video analysis of endoscopic cutting task performed by one versus two operators. Surg Endosc 19, 1388–1395 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00464-004-8209-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00464-004-8209-2

Keywords

Navigation