Abstract
Previous studies have reported bilingualism to be a proxy of cognitive reserve (CR) based on evidence that bilinguals express dementia symptoms ~ 4 years later than monolinguals yet present with greater neuropathology at time of diagnosis when clinical levels are similar. The current study provides new evidence supporting bilingualism’s contribution to CR using a novel brain health matching paradigm. Forty cognitively normal bilinguals with diffusion-weighted magnetic resonance images recruited from the community were matched with monolinguals drawn from a pool of 165 individuals in the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI) database. White matter integrity was determined for all participants using fractional anisotropy, axial diffusivity, and radial diffusivity scores. Propensity scores were obtained using white matter measures, sex, age, and education as predictive covariates, and then used in one-to-one matching between language groups, creating a matched sample of 32 participants per group. Matched monolinguals had poorer clinical diagnoses than that predicted by chance from a theoretical null distribution, and poorer cognitive performances than matched bilinguals as measured by scores on the MMSE. The findings provide support for the interpretation that bilingualism acts as a proxy of CR such that monolinguals have poorer clinical and cognitive outcomes than bilinguals for similar levels of white matter integrity even before clinical symptoms appear.
Similar content being viewed by others
Code availability
Available upon request.
References
Abutalebi J, Green DW (2008) Control mechanisms in bilingual language production: Neural evidence from language switching studies. Lang Cogn Process 23:557–582. https://doi.org/10.1080/016909608019206
Abutalebi J, Canini M, Della Rosa PA, Sheung LP, Green DW, Weekes BS (2014) Bilingualism protects anterior temporal lobe integrity in aging. Neurobiol Aging 35:2126–2133. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2014.03.010
Abutalebi J, Canini M, Della Rosa PA, Green DW, Weekes BS (2015) The neuroprotective effects of bilingualism upon the inferior parietal lobule: A structural neuroimaging study in aging Chinese bilinguals. J Neurolinguistics 33:3–13. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2014.09.008
Acosta-Cabronero J, Alley S, Williams GB, Pengas G, Nestor PJ (2012) Diffusion tensor metrics as biomarkers in Alzheimer’s disease. PLoS ONE 7:1–14. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0049072
Alladi S, Bak TH, Duggirala V et al (2013) Bilingualism delays age at onset of dementia, independent of education and immigration status. Neurol 81:1938–1944. https://doi.org/10.1212/01.wnl.0000436620.33155.a4
Anderson JAE, Chung-Fat-Yim A, Bellana B, Luk G, Bialystok E (2018a) Language and cognitive control networks in bilinguals and monolinguals. Neuropsychol 117:352–363. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.06.023
Anderson JAE, Grundy JG, Frutos JD, Barker RM, Grady C, Bialystok E (2018b) Effects of bilingualism on white matter integrity in older adults. NeuroImage 167:143–150. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.11.038
Anderson JAE, Mak L, Chahi AK, Bialystok E (2018c) The Language and Social Background Questionnaire: Assessing degree of bilingualism in a diverse population. Behav Res Methods 50:250–263. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-017-0867-9
Anderson JAE, Hawrylewicz K, Grundy JG (2020) Does bilingualism protect against dementia? A meta-analysis. Psychon Bull Rev 27:952–965
Austin PC (2011) An introduction to propensity score methods for reducing the effects of confounding in observational studies. Multivar Behav Res 46:399–424. https://doi.org/10.1080/00273171.2011.568786
Bennett IJ, Madden DJ (2014) Disconnected aging: Cerebral white matter integrity and age-related differences in cognition. Neurosci 276:187–205. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.11.026
Berkes M, Bialystok E, Craik FIM, Troyer A, Freedman M (2020) Conversion of mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer disease in monolingual and bilingual patients. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 34:225–230. https://doi.org/10.1097/WAD.0000000000000373
Bialystok E (2017) The bilingual adaptation: How minds accommodate experience. Psychol Bull 143:233–262. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000099
Bialystok E, Anderson JAE, Grundy JG (2018) Interpreting cognitive decline in the face of cognitive reserve: Does bilingualism affect cognitive aging? Linguist Approaches Biling. https://doi.org/10.1075/lab.18040.bia
Bialystok E, Craik FIM, Freedman M (2007) Bilingualism as a protection against the onset of symptoms of dementia. Neuropsychol 45:459–464. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.10.009
Bialystok E, Craik FIM, Luk G (2012) Bilingualism: Consequences for mind and brain. Trends Cogn Sci 16:240–250. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.03.001
Bialystok E, Abutalebi J, Bak TH, Burke DM, Kroll JF (2016) Aging in two languages: Implications for public health. Ageing Res Rev 27:56–60. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.arr.2016.03.003
Bosch B, Arenaza-Urquijo EM, Rami L et al (2012) Multiple DTI analysis in normal aging, amnestic MCI and AD Relationship with neuropsychological performance. Neurobiol Aging 33:61–74. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.02.004
Brini S, Sohrabi HR, Hebert JJ et al (2020) Bilingualism is associated with a delayed onset of dementia but not with a lower risk of developing it: A systematic review with meta-analyses. Neuropsychol Rev 30:1–24. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-020-09435-7
Burzynska AZ, Preuschhof C, Bäckman L et al (2010) Age-related differences in white matter microstructure: region-specific patterns of diffusivity. NeuroImage 49:2104–2112. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.09.041
Cabeza R, Albert M, Belleville S et al (2018) Cognitive neuroscience of healthy aging: Maintenance, reserve, and compensation. Nat Rev Neurosci 19:701–710. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41583-018-0068-2
Chertkow H, Whitehead V, Phillips N, Wolfson C, Atherton J, Bergman H (2010) Multilingualism (but not always bilingualism) delays the onset of Alzheimer disease: Evidence from a bilingual community. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 24:118–125. https://doi.org/10.1097/WAD.0b013e3181ca1221
Costumero V, Marin-Marin L, Calabria M et al (2020) A cross-sectional and longitudinal study on the protective effect of bilingualism against dementia using brain atrophy and cognitive measures. Alzheimer’s Res Ther. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13195-020-0581-1
Cox SR, Ritchie SJ, Tucker-Drob EM et al (2016) Ageing and brain white matter structure in 3,513 UK Biobank participants. Nat Commun 7:1–13. https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms13629
Del Maschio N, Fedeli D, Abutalebi J (2018) Bilingualism and aging: Why research should continue. Linguist Approaches Biling. https://doi.org/10.1075/lab.18032.del
Dick AS, Garcia NL, Pruden SM et al (2019) No evidence for a bilingual executive function advantage in the ABCD study. Nat Hum Behav 3:692–701. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-019-0609-3
Dijkstra T (2005) Bilingual word recognition and lexical access. In: Kroll JF, De Groot AMB (eds) Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approaches. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 179–201
Duñabeitia JA, Hernández JA, Antón E et al (2014) The inhibitory advantage in bilingual children revisited: Myth or reality? Exp Psychol 61:234–251. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000243
Fortin JP, Parker D, Tunç B et al (2017) Harmonization of multi-site diffusion tensor imaging data. NeuroImage 161:149–170. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.08.047
Gold BT, Johnson NF, Powell DK (2013) Lifelong bilingualism contributes to cognitive reserve against white matter integrity declines in aging. Neuropsychol 51:2841–2846. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.09.037
Grundy J, Anderson JAE (2017) Commentary: The relationship of bilingualism compared to monolingualism to the risk of cognitive decline or dementia: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Front Aging Neurosci 9:344. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2017.00344
Han L, Cole M, Bellavance F, McCusker J, Primeau F (2000) Tracking cognitive decline in Alzheimer’s disease using the mini-mental state examination: A meta-analysis. Int Psychogeriatr 12:231–247. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1041610200006359
Ho D, Imai K, King G, Stuart E (2007). Matching as nonparametric preprocessing for reducing model dependence in parametric causal inference. Polit Anal 15:199–236. http://gking.harvard.edu/files/abs/matchp-abs.shtml
Kroll J (2008) Juggling two languages in one mind. Psychol Sci Agenda 22:1–6
Kroll JF, Bobb S, Wodniecka Z (2006) Language selectivity is the exception, not the rule: Arguments against a fixed locus of language selection in bilingual speech. Biling Lang Cogn 9:119–135. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728906002483
Lawton DM, Gasquoine PG, Weimer AA (2015) Age of dementia diagnosis in community dwelling bilingual and monolingual Hispanic Americans. Cortex 66:141–145. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2014.11.017
Leivada E, Westergaard M, Duñabeitia JA, Rothman J (2020) On the phantom-like appearance of bilingualism effects on neurocognition: (How) should we proceed? Biling Lang Cogn. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728920000358
Lever J, Krzywinski M, Altman N (2017) Principal component analysis. Nat Methods 14:641–642. https://doi.org/10.1038/nmeth.4346
Luk G, Bialystok E, Craik FIM, Grady CL (2011a) Lifelong bilingualism maintains white matter integrity in older adults. J Neurosci 31:16808–16813. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.4563-11.2011
Luk G, Green DW, Abutalebi J, Grady C (2011b) Cognitive control for language switching in bilinguals: A quantitative meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. Lang Cogn Process 27:1479–1488. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2011.613209
Madden DJ, Bennett IJ, Song AW (2009) Cerebral white matter integrity and cognitive aging: Contributions from diffusion tensor imaging. Neuropsychol Rev 19:415–435. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-009-9113-2
Marian V, Spivey MJ (2003). Competing activation in bilingual language processing: Within- and between-language competition. Biling Lang Cogn 6:97–115. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728903001068
Meng X, D’Arcy C (2012) Education and dementia in the context of the cognitive reserve hypothesis: A systematic review with meta-analyses and qualitative analyses. PLoS ONE 7:e38268. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0038268
Michielse S, Coupland N, Camicioli R et al (2010) Selective effects of aging on brain white matter microstructure: A diffusion tensor imaging tractography study. NeuroImage 52:1190–1201. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.019
Mitchell A (2009) A meta-analysis of the accuracy of the mini-mental state examination in the detection of dementia and mild cognitive impairment. J Psychiatr Res 43:411–431. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2008.04.014
Mukadam N, Sommerlad A, Livingston G (2017) The relationship of bilingualism compared to monolingualism to the risk of cognitive decline or dementia: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J Alzheimer’s Dis 58:45–54. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-170131
Paap KR, Greenberg ZI (2013) There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cogn Psychol 66:232–258. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2012.12.002
Paap KR, Sawi O (2014) Bilingual advantages in executive functioning: Problems in convergent validity, discriminant validity, and the identification of the theoretical constructs. Front Psychol. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00962
Perani D, Farsad M, Ballarini T et al (2017) The impact of bilingualism on brain reserve and metabolic connectivity in Alzheimer’s dementia. PNAS 114:1690–1695. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1610909114
Pettigrew C, Soldan A, Zhu Y et al (2017) Cognitive reserve and cortical thickness in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Brain Imaging Behav 11:357–367. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-016-9581-y
Rosenbaum PR, Rubin DB (1983) The central role of the propensity score in observational studies for causal effects. Biometrika 70:41–55. https://doi.org/10.1093/biomet/70.1.41
Salat DH, Tuch DS, van der Kouwe AJW et al (2010) White matter pathology isolates the hippocampal formation in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging 31:244–256. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.03.013
Sanders AE, Hall CB, Katz MJ, Lipton RB (2012) Non-native language use and risk of incident dementia in the elderly. J Alzheimer’s Dis 29:99–108. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-2011-111631
Scarmeas N, Albert SM, Manly JJ, Stern Y (2006) Education and rates of cognitive decline in incident Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 77:308–316. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.2005.072306
Schilling KG, Blaber J, Huo Y et al (2019) Synthesized b0 for diffusion distortion correction (Synb0-DisCo). Magn Reson Imaging. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mri.2019.05.008
Schweizer TA, Ware J, Fischer CE, Craik FIM, Bialystok E (2012) Bilingualism as a contributor to cognitive reserve: Evidence from brain atrophy in Alzheimer’s disease. Cortex 48:991–996. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2011.04.009
Sexton CE, Walhovd KB, Storsve AB et al (2014) Accelerated changes in white matter microstructure during aging: A longitudinal diffusion tensor imaging study. J Neurosci 34:15425–15436. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0203-14.2014
Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Johansen-Berg H et al (2006) Tract-based spatial statistics: Voxelwise analysis of multi-subject diffusion data. NeuroImage 31:1487–1505. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.02.024
Stern Y (2002) What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 8:448–460. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617702813248
Stern Y (2009) Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychol 47:2015–2028. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.03.004
Stern Y, Albert S, Tang MX, Tsai WY (1999) Rate of memory decline in AD is related to education and occupation: Cognitive reserve? Neurology 53:1942–1947. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.53.9.1942
Von Bastian CC, Souza AS, Gade M (2016) No evidence for bilingual cognitive advantages: A test of four hypotheses. J Exp Psychol Gen 145:246–258. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000120d
Wilson RS, Boyle PA, Yang J, James BD, Bennett DA (2015) Early life instruction in foreign language and music and incidence of mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychol 29:292–302. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000129
Woumans E, Santens P, Sieben A, Versijpt J, Stevens M, Duyck W (2015) Bilingualism delays clinical manifestation of Alzheimer’s disease. Biling Lang Cogn 18:568–574. https://doi.org/10.1017/S136672891400087X
Zhang H, Wu YJ, Thierry G (2020) Bilingualism and aging: A focused neuroscientific review. J Neurolinguistics 54:100890. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2020.100890
Zheng Y, Wu Q, Su F, Fang Y, Zeng J, Pei Z (2018) The protective effect of Cantonese/Mandarin bilingualism on the onset of Alzheimer’s disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 45:210–219. https://doi.org/10.1159/000488485
Acknowledgements
Data used in preparation of this article were obtained from the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI) database (adni.loni.usc.edu). As such, the investigators within the ADNI contributed to the design and implementation of ADNI and/or provided data but did not participate in analysis or writing of this report. A complete listing of ADNI investigators can be found at: http://adni.loni.usc.edu/wp-content/uploads/how_to_apply/ADNI_Acknowledgement_List.pdf.
Funding
Preparation of this manuscript was supported by grants R01HD052523 and R21AG048431 from the US National Institutes of Health and grant A2559 from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada to EB.
Data collection and sharing for this project was funded by the Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI) (National Institutes of Health Grant U01 AG024904) and DOD ADNI (Department of Defense award number W81XWH-12-2-0012). ADNI is funded by the National Institute on Aging, the National Institute of Biomedical Imaging and Bioengineering, and through generous contributions from the following: AbbVie, Alzheimer’s Association; Alzheimer’s Drug Discovery Foundation; Araclon Biotech; BioClinica, Inc.; Biogen; Bristol-Myers Squibb Company; CereSpir, Inc.; Cogstate; Eisai Inc.; Elan Pharmaceuticals, Inc.; Eli Lilly and Company; EuroImmun; F. Hoffmann-La Roche Ltd and its affiliated company Genentech, Inc.; Fujirebio; GE Healthcare; IXICO Ltd.; Janssen Alzheimer Immunotherapy Research & Development, LLC.; Johnson & Johnson Pharmaceutical Research & Development LLC.; Lumosity; Lundbeck; Merck & Co., Inc.; Meso Scale Diagnostics, LLC.; NeuroRx Research; Neurotrack Technologies; Novartis Pharmaceuticals Corporation; Pfizer Inc.; Piramal Imaging; Servier; Takeda Pharmaceutical Company; and Transition Therapeutics. The Canadian Institutes of Health Research is providing funds to support ADNI clinical sites in Canada. Private sector contributions are facilitated by the Foundation for the National Institutes of Health (www.fnih.org). The grantee organization is the Northern California Institute for Research and Education, and the study is coordinated by the Alzheimer’s Therapeutic Research Institute at the University of Southern California. ADNI data are disseminated by the Laboratory for Neuro Imaging at the University of Southern California.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Consortia
Contributions
Matthias Berkes, Noelia Calvo, and Ellen Bialystok contributed to the study conception and design. Material preparation, data collection, and analysis were performed by Matthias Berkes, Noelia Calvo, and John A. E. Anderson. The first draft of the manuscript was written by Matthias Berkes and all authors commented on previous versions of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Conflict of interest
None.
Ethics approval
The research study was approved by York University’s Ethics Review Board and conforms to the standards of the Canadian Tri-Council Research Ethics guidelines.
Consent to participate
Bilingual participants signed written consent before the study and verbal assent throughout out. Monolingual participants were selected from the ADNI database where consent procedures were handled by ADNI.
Consent for publication
All authors consent to publication.
Additional information
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Berkes, M., Calvo, N., Anderson, J.A.E. et al. Poorer clinical outcomes for older adult monolinguals when matched to bilinguals on brain health. Brain Struct Funct 226, 415–424 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-020-02185-5
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-020-02185-5