Bakdash, J. Z., & Marusich, L. R. (2017). Repeated measures correlation. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 456.
Article
Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F., Mesquita, B., & Gendron, M. (2011). Context in emotion perception. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20(5), 286–290. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721411422522
Article
Google Scholar
Batista, D., Plácido da Silva, H., Fred, A., Moreira, C., Reis, M., & Ferreira, H. A. (2019). Benchmarking of the BITalino biomedical toolkit against an established gold standard. Healthcare Technology Letters, 6(2), 32–36. https://doi.org/10.1049/htl.2018.5037
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Batista, D., Silva, H., & Fred, A. (2017). Experimental characterization and analysis of the BITalino platforms against a reference device. 2017 39th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC), 2418–2421. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1109/EMBC.2017.8037344
Benedek, M., & Kaernbach, C. (2010). A continuous measure of phasic electrodermal activity. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 190(1), 80–91. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2010.04.028
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Benjamin, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (2009). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, 57(1), 289–300. https://doi.org/10.2307/2346101
Article
Google Scholar
Bernstein, A. S. (1965). Race and examiner as significant influences on basal skin impedance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1(4), 346–349. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0021747
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Boucsein, W. (2012). Methods of Electrodermal Recording. In Electrodermal Activity (2nd ed., pp. 208–213, 380–381). New York: Springer
Boucsein, W., Fowles, D. C., Grimnes, S., Ben-Shakhar, G., Roth, W. T., Dawson, M. E., & Filion, D. L. (2012). Publication recommendations for electrodermal measurements. Psychophysiology, 49(8), 1017–1034. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2012.01384.x
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Briefer, E. F. (2012). Vocal expression of emotions in mammals: Mechanisms of production and evidence. Journal of Zoology, 288(1), 1–20. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7998.2012.00920.x
Article
Google Scholar
Cohen, J. (1977). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Academic Press.
Google Scholar
Cordaro, D. T., Keltner, D., Tshering, S., Wangchuk, D., & Flynn, L. M. (2016). The voice conveys emotion in ten globalized cultures and one remote village in Bhutan. Emotion, 16(1), 117–128. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000100
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
da Silva, H. P., Fred, A., & Martins, R. (2014). Biosignals for Everyone. IEEE Pervasive Computing, 13(4), 64–71. https://doi.org/10.1109/MPRV.2014.61
Article
Google Scholar
Davila Ross, M., Owren, J., & M., & Zimmermann, E. . (2009). Reconstructing the evolution of laughter in great apes and humans. Current Biology, 19(13), 1106–1111. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.CUB.2009.05.028
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Dawson, M. E., Schell, A. M., & Filion, D. L. (2007). The Electrodermal System. In J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary, & G. G. Berntson (Eds.), Handbook of Psychophysiology (3rd ed., pp. 159–181). https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1017/9781107415782
Ducci, L. (1981). Reaction times in the recognition of facial expressions of emotion. Italian Journal of Psychology, 8(3), 183–193.
Google Scholar
Eastwood, J. D., Smilek, D., & Merikle, P. M. (2003). Negative facial expression captures attention and disrupts performance. Perception & Psychophysics, 65(3), 352–358. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03194566
Article
Google Scholar
Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. (2002a). Is there an in-group advantage in emotion recognition? Psychological Bulletin, 128(2), 243–249. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.128.2.243
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. (2002b). On the universality and cultural specificity of emotion recognition: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 128(2), 203–235. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.128.2.203
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Elfenbein, H. A., Beaupré, M., Lévesque, M., & Hess, U. (2007). Toward a dialect theory: Cultural differences in the expression and recognition of posed facial expressions. Emotion, 7(1), 131–146. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.7.1.131
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Engström, J., Johansson, E., & Östlund, J. (2005). Effects of visual and cognitive load in real and simulated motorway driving. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 8(2), 97–120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2005.04.012
Article
Google Scholar
F Johnson, W., Emde, R., Scherer, K., & Klinnert, M. (1986). Recognition of emotion from vocal cues. In Archives of general psychiatry (Vol. 43).
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A.-G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 1149–1160. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149
Article
Google Scholar
Fitzpatrick, S., Ip, J., Krantz, L., Zeifman, R., & Kuo, J. R. (2019). Use your words: The role of emotion labeling in regulating emotion in borderline personality disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 120, 103447. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2019.103447
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Fowles, D. C., Christie, M. J., Edelberg, R., Grings, W. W., Lykken, D. T., & Venables, P. H. (2012). Publication recommendations for electrodermal measurements. Psychophysiology, 49(8), 1017–1034. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2012.01384.x
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions perspectives on emotions and affect NIH. American Psychologist, 56(3), 218–226.
Article
Google Scholar
Gendron, M., Roberson, D., van der Vyver, J. M., & Barrett, L. F. (2014). Cultural relativity in perceiving emotion from vocalizations. Psychological Science, 25(4), 911–920. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613517239
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Gervais, M., & Wilson, D. S. (2005). The Evolution and functions of laughter and humor: A synthetic approach. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 80(4), 395–430. https://doi.org/10.1086/498281
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Grandjean, D., Sander, D., Pourtois, G., Schwartz, S., Seghier, M. L., Scherer, K. R., & Vuilleumier, P. (2005). The voices of wrath: brain responses to angry prosody in meaningless speech. Nature Neuroscience, 8(2), 145–146. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1392
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Haidt, J., & Keltner, D. (1999). Culture and facial expression: Open-ended methods find more expressions and a gradient of recognition. Cognition and Emotion, 13(3), 225–266. https://doi.org/10.1080/026999399379267
Article
Google Scholar
Hubbard, A. E., Ahern, J., Fleischer, N. L., Laan, M. V., Der, L., & S. A., Jewell, N., … Satariano, W. A. . (2010). To GEE or not to GEE: Comparing population average and mixed models for estimating the associations between neighborhood risk factors and health. Epidemiology, 21(4), 467–474. https://doi.org/10.1097/EDE.0b013e3181caeb90
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Hugenberg, K. (2005). Social categorization and the perception of facial affect: Target race moderates the response latency advantage for happy faces. Emotion, 5, 267–276. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.5.3.267
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Jack, R. E., Blais, C., Scheepers, C., Schyns, P. G., & Caldara, R. (2009). Cultural confusions show that facial expressions are not universal. Current Biology, 19(18), 1543–1548. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2009.07.051
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Janes, C. L., Hesselbrock, V., & Stern, J. A. (1978). Parental psychopathology, age, and race as related to electrodermal activity of children. Psychophysiology, 15(1), 24–34. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1978.tb01329.x
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Johnson, L. C., & Landon, M. M. (1965). Eccrine sweat gland activity and racial differences in resting skin conductance. Psychophysiology, 1(4), 322–329. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1965.tb03264.x
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Juniper, K., Dykman, R., & a. . (1967). Skin resistance, sweat-gland counts, salivary flow, and gastric secretion: Age, race, and sex differences, and intercorrelations. Psychophysiology, 4(2), 216–222.
Article
Google Scholar
Jürgens, R., Drolet, M., Pirow, R., Scheiner, E., & Fischer, J. (2013). Encoding conditions affect recognition of vocally expressed emotions across cultures. Frontiers in psychology. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00111
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Juslin, P. N., & Laukka, P. (2001). Impact of intended emotion intensity on cue utilization and decoding accuracy in vocal expression of emotion. Emotion, 1(4), 381–412. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.1.4.381
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Kahneman, D., & Beatty, J. (1966). Pupil diameter and load on memory. Science, 154(3756), 1583–1585. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.154.3756.1583
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Khalfa, S., Isabelle, P., Jean-Pierre, B., & Manon, R. (2002). Event-related skin conductance responses to musical emotions in humans. Neuroscience Letters, 328(2), 145–149. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3940(02)00462-7
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Kim, K. H., Bang, S. W., & Kim, S. R. (2004). Emotion recognition system using short-term monitoring of physiological signals. Medical and Biological Engineering and Computing, 42(3), 419–427. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02344719
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Kirita, T., & Endo, M. (1995). Happy face advantage in recognizing facial expressions. Acta Psychologica, 89(2), 149–163. https://doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(94)00021-8
Article
Google Scholar
Koeda, M., Belin, P., Hama, T., Masuda, T., Matsuura, M., & Okubo, Y. (2013). Cross-cultural differences in the processing of non-verbal affective vocalizations by japanese and canadian listeners. Frontiers in Psychology. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00105
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Lang, P. J., Greenwald, M. K., Bradley, M. M., & Hamm, A. O. (1993). Looking at pictures: affective, facial, visceral, and behavioral reactions. Psychophysiology, 30(3), 261–273. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1993.tb03352.x
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Laukka, P., Elfenbein, H. A., Söder, N., Nordström, H., Althoff, J., Chui, W., et al. (2013). Cross-cultural decoding of positive and negative non-linguistic emotion vocalizations. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 1–8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00353
Article
Google Scholar
Lausen, A., & Schacht, A. (2018). Gender differences in the recognition of vocal emotions. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 882.
Article
Google Scholar
Leppänen, J. M., & Hietanen, J. K. (2004). Positive facial expressions are recognized faster than negative facial expressions, but why? Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 69(1–2), 22–29. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-003-0157-2
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Leppänen, J. M., Tenhunen, M., & Hietanen, J. K. (2003). Faster choice-reaction times to positive than to negative facial expressions: The role of cognitive and motor processes. Journal of Psychophysiology, 17(3), 113–123. https://doi.org/10.1027//0269-8803.17.3.113
Article
Google Scholar
Lima, C. F., Castro, S. L., & Scott, S. K. (2013). When voices get emotional: A corpus of nonverbal vocalizations for research on emotion processing. Behavior Research Methods, 45(4), 1234–1245.
Article
Google Scholar
Lykken, D. T., Rose, R., Luther, B., & Maley, M. (1966). Correcting psychophysiological measures for individual differences in range. Psychological Bulletin, 66, 481–484. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0023922
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
MacPherson, M. K., Abur, D., & Stepp, C. E. (2017). Acoustic measures of voice and physiologic measures of autonomic arousal during speech as a function of cognitive load. Journal of Voice, 31(4), 504.e1-504.e9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvoice.2016.10.021
Article
Google Scholar
Matsumoto, D. (2002). Methodological requirements to test a possible in-group advantage in judging emotions across cultures: Comment on Elfenbein and Ambady (2002) and evidence. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 236–242. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.128.2.236
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Measures, S., & for P. R. A. H. C. on E. . (2012). Publication recommendations for electrodermal measurements. Psychophysiology, 49(8), 1017–1034. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2012.01384.x
Article
Google Scholar
Mehler, B., Reimer, B., & Coughlin, J. F. (2012). Sensitivity of physiological measures for detecting systematic variations in cognitive demand from a working memory task: An on-road study across three age groups. Human Factors, 54(3), 396–412. https://doi.org/10.1177/0018720812442086
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Mesquita, B., & Frijda, N. H. (1992). Cultural variations in emotions: A review. 112(September 1992), 179–204. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-2909.112.2.179
Miller, J. C. (1975). The politics of decolonization in Portuguese Africa. African Affairs, 74(295), 135–147. Retrieved from https://www.jstor.org/stable/721179
Niedenthal, P. M., Rychlowska, M., & Wood, A. (2017). Feelings and contexts: socioecological influences on the nonverbal expression of emotion. Current Opinion in Psychology, 17, 170–175. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.07.025
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Nourbakhsh, N., Chen, F., Wang, Y., & Calvo, R. A. (2017). Detecting users’ cognitive load by galvanic skin response with affective interference. ACM Transactions on Interactive Intelligent Systems, 7(3), 12:1-12:20. https://doi.org/10.1145/2960413
Article
Google Scholar
Paul Ekman. (1999). Handbook of Cognition and Emotion (M. P. T. Dalgleish, Ed.). Wiley, New York
Peirce, J. (2009). Generating stimuli for neuroscience using PsychoPy. Frontiers in Neuroinformatics, 2, 10.
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Petrone, C., Carbone, F., & Champagne-Lavau, M. (2016). Effects of emotional prosody on skin conductance responses in French. Proceedings of the 8th International Conference on Speech Prosody (SP2016), 425–429. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.21437/SpeechProsody.2016-87
Peysakhovich, V., Causse, M., Scannella, S., & Dehais, F. (2015). Frequency analysis of a task-evoked pupillary response: Luminance-independent measure of mental effort. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 97(1), 30–37. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.04.019
R Core Team. (2014). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Retrieved from http://www.r-project.org/
Richardson, J. T. E. (2011). January). Eta squared and partial eta squared as measures of effect size in educational research. Educational Research Review, 6, 135–147. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.edurev.2010.12.001
Article
Google Scholar
Rosa, P. J., Oliveira, J., Alghazzawi, D., Fardoun, H., & Gamito, P. (2017). Affective and physiological correlates of the perception of unimodal and bimodal emotional stimuli. Psicothema, 29(3), 364–369. https://doi.org/10.7334/psicothema2016.272
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Ruch, W., & Ekman, P. (2001). The expressive pattern of laughter. In A. Kaszniak (Ed.), Emotion, QualIA AND CONsciousness (pp. 426–443). Word Scientific Publisher.
Chapter
Google Scholar
Russell, J. A. (1994). Is there universal recognition of emotion from facial expression? A review of the cross-cultural studies. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 102–141.
Article
Google Scholar
Sander, D., Grandjean, D., Pourtois, G., Schwartz, S., Seghier, M. L., Scherer, K. R., & Vuilleumier, P. (2005). Emotion and attention interactions in social cognition: Brain regions involved in processing anger prosody. NeuroImage, 28(4), 848–858. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.06.023
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Sauter, E., & F., Ekman, P., & Scott, S. K. . (2010). Cross-cultural recognition of basic emotions through nonverbal emotional vocalizations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(6), 2408–2412. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0908239106
Article
Google Scholar
Sauter, & Scott, S. K. . (2007). More than one kind of happiness: Can we recognize vocal expressions of different positive states? Motivation and Emotion, 31(3), 192–199. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-007-9065-x
Article
Google Scholar
Scherer, K. R., Banse, R., & Wallbott, H. G. (2001). Emotion Inferences from Vocal Expression Correlate Across Languages and Cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 32(1), 76–92. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022101032001009
Article
Google Scholar
Schröder, M. (2003). Experimental study of affect bursts. Speech Communication, 40, 99–116.
Article
Google Scholar
Scott, S. K., Lavan, N., Chen, S., & McGettigan, C. (2014). The social life of laughter. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(12), 618–620. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.09.002
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Shi, Y., Ruiz, N., Taib, R., Choi, E., & Chen, F. (2007). Galvanic Skin Response (GSR) as an Index of Cognitive Load. CHI ’07 Extended Abstracts on Human Factors in Computing Systems, 2651–2656. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1145/1240866.1241057
Shiota, M. N., Campos, B., Keltner, D., & Hertenstein, M. J. (2004). Positive emotion and the regulation of interpersonal relationships. The regulation of emotion (pp. 127–155). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
Google Scholar
Siegle, G. J., Steinhauer, S. R., Stenger, V. A., Konecky, R., & Carter, C. S. (2003). Use of concurrent pupil dilation assessment to inform interpretation and analysis of fMRI data. NeuroImage, 20(1), 114–124. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00298-2
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Simon-Thomas, E. R., Keltner, D. J., Sauter, D., Sinicropi-Yao, L., & Abramson, A. (2009). The voice conveys specific emotions: evidence from vocal burst displays. Emotion, 9(6), 838–846. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017810
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Skuse, D. (2003). Fear recognition and the neural basis of social cognition. Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 8(2), 50–60. https://doi.org/10.1111/1475-3588.00047
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Skwerer, D. P., Borum, L., Verbalis, A., Schofield, C., Crawford, N., Ciciolla, L., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2009). Autonomic responses to dynamic displays of facial expressions in adolescents and adults with Williams syndrome. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 4(1), 93–100. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsn041
Article
Google Scholar
Stenberg, G., Wiking, S., & Dahl, M. (1998). Judging words at face value: interference in a word processing task reveals automatic processing of affective facial expressions. Cognition and Emotion, 12(6), 755–782. https://doi.org/10.1080/026999398379420
Article
Google Scholar
ten Brinke, L., & Porter, S. (2012). Cry me a river: Identifying the behavioral consequences of extremely high-stakes interpersonal deception. Law and Human Behavior, 36, 469–477. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0093929
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Thompson, W. F., & Balkwill, L. L. (2006). Decoding speech prosody in five languages. In Semiotica (Vol. 158). https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1515/SEM.2006.017
Tracy, J. L., & Randles, D. (2011). Four models of basic emotions: A review of Ekman and Cordaro, Izard, Levenson, and Panksepp and Watt. Emotion Review, 3(4), 397–405. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073911410747
Article
Google Scholar
van der Wel, P., & van Steenbergen, H. (2018). Pupil dilation as an index of effort in cognitive control tasks: A review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(6), 2005–2015. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-018-1432-y
Article
Google Scholar
Venables, P. H., Gartshore, S. A., & O’Riordan, P. W. (1980). The function of skin conductance response recovery and rise time. Biological Psychology, 10(1), 1–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/0301-0511(80)90002-2
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Vettin, J., & Todt, D. (2005). Human laughter, social play, and play vocalizations of non-human primates: An evolutionary approach. Behaviour, 142(2), 217–240.
Article
Google Scholar
Wang, C.-A., Baird, T., Huang, J., Coutinho, J. D., Brien, D. C., & Munoz, D. P. (2018). Arousal effects on pupil size, heart rate, and skin conductance in an emotional face task. Frontiers in Neurology, 9, 1029. https://doi.org/10.3389/fneur.2018.01029
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Wilson-Mendenhall, C. D., Barrett, L. F., Simmons, W. K., & Barsalou, L. W. (2011). Grounding emotion in situated conceptualization. Neuropsychologia, 49(5), 1105–1127. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.12.032
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Wolfensberger, W., & O’Connor, N. (1967). Relative effectiveness of galvanic skin response latency, amplitude and duration scores as measures of arousal and habituation in normal and retarded adults. Psychophysiology, 3(4), 345–350. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1967.tb02718.x
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Zénon, A., Sidibé, M., & Olivier, E. (2014). Pupil size variations correlate with physical effort perception. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 286. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00286
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar