Aarts, H., Bijleveld, E., Custers, R., Dogge, M., Deelder, M., Schutter, D., et al. (2012). Positive priming and intentional binding: Eye-blink rate predicts reward information effects on the sense of agency. Social Neuroscience, 7(1), 105–112. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2011.590602.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Arkin, R. M., Appelman, A. J., & Burger, J. M. (1980). Social anxiety, self-presentation, and the self-serving bias in causal attribution. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38(1), 23–35. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.38.1.23.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Barlas, Z., Hockley, W. E., & Obhi, S. S. (2017). The effects of freedom of choice in action selection on perceived mental effort and the sense of agency. Acta Psychologica, 180(August), 122–129. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2017.09.004.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Barlas, Z., & Kopp, S. (2018). Action choice and outcome congruency independently affect intentional binding and feeling of control judgments. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 12(April), 1–10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2018.00137.
Article
Google Scholar
Barlas, Z., & Obhi, S. S. (2013). Freedom, choice, and the sense of agency. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7(August), 514. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00514.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Barlas, Z., Hockley, W. E., & Obhi, S. S. (2018). Effects of free choice and outcome valence on the sense of agency: evidence from measures of intentional binding and feelings of control. Experimental Brain Research, 236(1), 129–139. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-017-5112-3.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Beck, B., Di Costa, S., & Haggard, P. (2017). Having control over the external world increases the implicit sense of agency. Cognition, 162, 54–60. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2017.02.002.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Blakemore, S.-J., & Frith, C. D. (2003). Self-awareness and action. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 13(2), 219–224. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0959-4388(03)00043-6.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Blakemore, S.-J., & Sirigu, A. (2003). Action prediction in the cerebellum and in the parietal lobe. Experimental Brain Research, 153(2), 239–245. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-003-1597-z.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Blakemore, S.-J., Wolpert, D. M., & Frith, C. D. (2002). Abnormalities in the awareness of action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(6), 237–242. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01907-1.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Borhani, K., Beck, B., & Haggard, P. (2017). Choosing, doing, and controlling: implicit sense of agency over somatosensory events. Psychological Science, 28(7), 882–893. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797617697693.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
van den Bos, E., & Jeannerod, M. (2002). Sense of body and sense of action both contribute to self-recognition. Cognition, 85(2), 177–187. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00100-2.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Braun, N., Thorne, J. D., Hildebrandt, H., & Debener, S. (2014). interplay of agency and ownership: the intentional binding and rubber hand illusion paradigm combined. PLoS One, 9(11), e111967. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0111967.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Brugada-Ramentol, V., Clemens, I., & de Polavieja, G. G. (2019). Active control as evidence in favor of sense of ownership in the moving Virtual hand illusion. Consciousness and Cognition, 71(2018), 123–135. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2019.04.003.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Burin, D., Garbarini, F., Bruno, V., Fossataro, C., Destefanis, C., Berti, A., et al. (2017). Movements and body ownership: evidence from the rubber hand illusion after mechanical limb immobilization. Neuropsychologia, 107(2018), 41–47. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.11.004.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Burin, D., Livelli, A., Garbarini, F., Fossataro, C., Folegatti, A., Gindri, P., et al. (2015). Are movements necessary for the sense of body ownership? Evidence from the rubber hand illusion in pure hemiplegic patients. PLoS One, 10(3), 1–12. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0117155.
Article
Google Scholar
Campbell, J. I. D., & Thompson, V. A. (2012). MorePower 6.0 for ANOVA with relational confidence intervals and Bayesian analysis. Behavior Research Methods, 44(4), 1255–1265. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-012-0186-0.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Caspar, E. A., Christensen, J. F., Cleeremans, A., & Haggard, P. (2016). Coercion changes the sense of agency in the human brain. Current Biology, 26(5), 585–592. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.12.067.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Caspar, E. A., Cleeremans, A., & Haggard, P. (2015). The relationship between human agency and embodiment. Consciousness and Cognition, 33, 226–236. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2015.01.007.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Caspar, E. A., Cleeremans, A., & Haggard, P. (2018). Only giving orders? An experimental study of the sense of agency when giving or receiving commands. PLoS One, 13(9), e0204027. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0204027.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Caspar, E. A., Desantis, A., Dienes, Z., Cleeremans, A., & Haggard, P. (2016). The sense of agency as tracking control. PLoS One, 11(10), e0163892. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0163892.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Caspar, E. A., Vuillaume, L., De Saldanha, M., da Gama, P. A., & Cleeremans, A. (2017). The influence of (Dis)belief in free will on immoral behavior. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1–9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00020.
Article
Google Scholar
Danielmeier, C., & Ullsperger, M. (2011). Post-error adjustments. Frontiers in Psychology. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00233.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Daprati, E., Franck, N., Georgieff, N., Proust, J., Pacherie, E., Dalery, J., et al. (1997). Looking for the agent: an investigation into consciousness of action and self-consciousness in schizophrenic patients. Cognition, 65(1), 71–86. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(97)00039-5.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
David, N., Skoruppa, S., Gulberti, A., Schultz, J., & Engel, A. K. (2016). The sense of agency is more sensitive to manipulations of outcome than movement-related feedback irrespective of sensory modality. PLoS One, 11(8), e0161156. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0161156.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Desantis, A., Hughes, G., & Waszak, F. (2012). Intentional binding is driven by the mere presence of an action and not by motor prediction. PLoS One, 7(1), e29557. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0029557.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Farrer, C., Frey, S. H., Van Horn, J. D., Tunik, E., Turk, D., Inati, S., et al. (2008). The angular Gyrus computes action awareness representations. Cerebral Cortex, 18(2), 254–261. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhm050.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Fischer, R. (2004). Standardization to account for cross-cultural response bias. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 35(3), 263–282. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022104264122.
Article
Google Scholar
Fischer, R., & Milfont, T. L. (2010). Standardization in psychological research. International Journal of Psychological Research, 3(1 SE-Research Article), 5. https://doi.org/10.21500/20112084.852.
Article
Google Scholar
Fourneret, P., & Jeannerod, M. (1998). Limited conscious monitoring of motor performance in normal subjects. Neuropsychologia, 36(11), 1133–1140. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(98)00006-2.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Franck, N., Farrer, C., Georgieff, N., Marie-Cardine, M., Daléry, J., D’Amato, T., et al. (2001). Defective recognition of one’s own actions in patients with schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158(3), 454–459. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.158.3.454.
Article
Google Scholar
Fried, I., Haggard, P., He, B. J., & Schurger, A. (2017). Volition and action in the human brain: processes, pathologies, and reasons. The Journal of Neuroscience, 37(45), 10842–10847. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2584-17.2017.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Friston, K. (2010). The free-energy principle: a unified brain theory? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(2), 127–138. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2787.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Friston, K. (2011). What Is optimal about motor control? Neuron, 72(3), 488–498. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2011.10.018.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Friston, K. (2012). Prediction, perception and agency. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 83(2), 248–252. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.11.014.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Friston, K., Samothrakis, S., & Montague, R. (2012). Active inference and agency: optimal control without cost functions. Biological Cybernetics, 106(8–9), 523–541. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00422-012-0512-8.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Frith, C. D. (2013). The psychology of volition. Experimental Brain Research, 229(3), 289–299. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-013-3407-6.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Frith, C. D., Blakemore, S.-J., & Wolpert, D. M. (2000). Abnormalities in the awareness and control of action. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 355(1404), 1771–1788. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2000.0734.
Article
Google Scholar
Fusco, G., Scandola, M., Feurra, M., Pavone, E. F., Rossi, S., & Aglioti, S. M. (2018). Midfrontal theta transcranial alternating current stimulation modulates behavioural adjustment after error execution. European Journal of Neuroscience, 48(10), 3159–3170. https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.14174.
Article
Google Scholar
Gentsch, A., Ullsperger, P., & Ullsperger, M. (2009). Dissociable medial frontal negativities from a common monitoring system for self- and externally caused failure of goal achievement. NeuroImage, 47(4), 2023–2030. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.05.064.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Haering, C., & Kiesel, A. (2014). Intentional binding is independent of the validity of the action effect’s identity. Acta Psychologica, 152, 109–119. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.07.015.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Haggard, P. (2017). Sense of agency in the human brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 18(4), 196–207. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2017.14.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Haggard, P., Clark, S., & Kalogeras, J. (2002). Voluntary action and conscious awareness. Nature Neuroscience, 5(4), 382–385. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn827.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Kalckert, A., & Ehrsson, H. H. (2012). Moving a rubber hand that feels like your own: a dissociation of ownership and agency. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6(March), 1–14. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00040.
Article
Google Scholar
Kalckert, A., & Ehrsson, H. H. (2014). The moving rubber hand illusion revisited: comparing movements and visuotactile stimulation to induce illusory ownership. Consciousness and Cognition, 26(1), 117–132. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2014.02.003.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Kirsch, W., Kunde, W., & Herbort, O. (2019). Intentional binding is unrelated to action intention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 45(3), 378–385. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000612.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Kühn, S., Nenchev, I., Haggard, P., Brass, M., Gallinat, J., & Voss, M. (2011). Whodunnit? Electrophysiological Correlates of Agency Judgements. PLoS One, 6(12), e28657. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0028657.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Miller, D. T., & Ross, M. (1975). Self-serving biases in the attribution of causality: fact or fiction? Psychological Bulletin, 82(2), 213–225. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0076486.
Article
Google Scholar
Monti, A., Porciello, G., Tieri, G., & Aglioti, S. M. (2020). The “embreathment” illusion highlights the role of breathing in corporeal awareness. Journal of Neurophysiology, 123(1), 420–427. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00617.2019.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Moore, J. W., & Fletcher, P. C. (2012). Sense of agency in health and disease: A review of cue integration approaches. Consciousness and Cognition, 21(1), 59–68. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2011.08.010.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Moore, J. W., & Obhi, S. S. (2012). Intentional binding and the sense of agency: a review. Consciousness and Cognition, 21(1), 546–561. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2011.12.002.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Moore, J. W., Lagnado, D., Deal, D. C., & Haggard, P. (2009). Feelings of control: contingency determines experience of action. Cognition, 110(2), 279–283. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2008.11.006.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Moreau, Q., Candidi, M., Era, V., Tieri, G., & Aglioti, S. M. (2020). Midline frontal and occipito-temporal activity during error monitoring in dyadic motor interactions. Cortex, (February), 1–19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2020.01.020.
Article
Google Scholar
Osumi, M., Nobusako, S., Zama, T., Yokotani, N., Shimada, S., Maeda, T., et al. (2019). The relationship and difference between delay detection ability and judgment of sense of agency. PLoS One, 14(7), e0219222. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0219222.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
den Ouden, H. E. M., Kok, P., & de Lange, F. P. (2012). How prediction errors shape perception, attention, and motivation. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 1–12. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00548.
Article
Google Scholar
Padrao, G., Gonzalez-Franco, M., Sanchez-Vives, M. V., Slater, M., & Rodriguez-Fornells, A. (2016). Violating body movement semantics: neural signatures of self-generated and external-generated errors. NeuroImage, 124, 147–156. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.08.022.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Pavone, E. F., Tieri, G., Rizza, G., Tidoni, E., Grisoni, L., & Aglioti, S. M. (2016). Embodying others in immersive virtual reality: electro-cortical signatures of monitoring the errors in the actions of an avatar seen from a first-person perspective. Journal of Neuroscience, 36(2), 268–279. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0494-15.2016.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Pezzetta, R., Nicolardi, V., Tidoni, E., & Aglioti, S. M. (2018). Error, rather than its probability, elicits specific electrocortical signatures: a combined EEG-immersive virtual reality study of action observation. Journal of Neurophysiology, 120(3), 1107–1118. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00130.2018.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Poonian, S. K., & Cunnington, R. (2013). Intentional binding in self-made and observed actions. Experimental Brain Research, 229(3), 419–427. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-013-3505-5.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Salomon, R., Fernandez, N. B., Van Elk, M., Vachicouras, N., Sabatier, F., Tychinskaya, A., et al. (2016). Changing motor perception by sensorimotor conflicts and body ownership. Scientific Reports, 6(2015), 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep25847.
Article
Google Scholar
Sato, A., & Yasuda, A. (2005). Illusion of sense of self-agency: discrepancy between the predicted and actual sensory consequences of actions modulates the sense of self-agency, but not the sense of self-ownership. Cognition, 94(3), 241–255. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2004.04.003.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Shanks, D. R., Pearson, S. M., & Dickinson, A. (1989). Temporal contiguity and the judgement of causality by human subjects. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section B, 41(2), 139–159. https://doi.org/10.1080/14640748908401189.
Article
Google Scholar
Suzuki, K., Lush, P., Seth, A. K., & Roseboom, W. (2019). Intentional binding without intentional action. Psychological Science, 30(6), 842–853. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797619842191.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Synofzik, M., Vosgerau, G., & Newen, A. (2008a). Beyond the comparator model: a multifactorial two-step account of agency. Consciousness and Cognition, 17(1), 219–239. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2007.03.010.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Synofzik, M., Vosgerau, G., & Newen, A. (2008b). I move, therefore I am: a new theoretical framework to investigate agency and ownership. Consciousness and Cognition, 17(2), 411–424. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2008.03.008.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Synofzik, M., Vosgerau, G., & Voss, M. (2013). The experience of agency: an interplay between prediction and postdiction. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(MAR), 1–8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00127.
Article
Google Scholar
Takahata, K., Takahashi, H., Maeda, T., Umeda, S., Suhara, T., Mimura, M., et al. (2012). It’s not my fault: postdictive modulation of intentional binding by monetary gains and losses. PLoS One, 7(12), 1–8. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0053421.
Article
Google Scholar
Tieri, G., Tidoni, E., Pavone, E. F., & Aglioti, S. M. (2015). Mere observation of body discontinuity affects perceived ownership and vicarious agency over a virtual hand. Experimental Brain Research, 233(4), 1247–1259. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-015-4202-3.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Tsakiris, M., Longo, M. R., & Haggard, P. (2010). Having a body versus moving your body: Neural signatures of agency and body-ownership. Neuropsychologia, 48(9), 2740–2749. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.05.021.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Ullsperger, M., Danielmeier, C., & Jocham, G. (2014). Neurophysiology of performance monitoring and adaptive behavior. Physiological Reviews, 94(1), 35–79. https://doi.org/10.1152/physrev.00041.2012.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Villa, R., Tidoni, E., Porciello, G., & Aglioti, S. M. (2018). Violation of expectations about movement and goal achievement leads to sense of agency reduction. Experimental Brain Research, 236(7), 2123–2135. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-018-5286-3.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Wegner, D. M., & Sparrow, B. (2004). Authorship processing. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The New Cognitive Neurosciences (3rd ed., pp. 1201–1209). Cambridge: MIT Press.
Google Scholar
Wegner, D. M., Sparrow, B., & Winerman, L. (2004). Vicarious agency: experiencing control over the movements of others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86(6), 838–848. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.86.6.838.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Wegner, D. M., & Wheatley, T. (1999). Apparent mental causation: sources of the experience of will. American Psychologist, 54(7), 480–492. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.54.7.480.
Article
Google Scholar
Weiss, C., Tsakiris, M., Haggard, P., & Schütz-Bosbach, S. (2014). Agency in the sensorimotor system and its relation to explicit action awareness. Neuropsychologia, 52(1), 82–92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.09.034.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Wenke, D., Fleming, S. M., & Haggard, P. (2010). Subliminal priming of actions influences sense of control over effects of action. Cognition, 115(1), 26–38. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.10.016.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Wessel, J. R., & Aron, A. R. (2013). Unexpected events induce motor slowing via a brain mechanism for action-stopping with global suppressive effects. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(47), 18481–18491. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3456-13.2013.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Wessel, J. R., Danielmeier, C., Morton, J. B., & Ullsperger, M. (2012). Surprise and error: common neuronal architecture for the processing of errors and novelty. Journal of Neuroscience, 32(22), 7528–7537. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6352-11.2012.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Wolpe, N., & Rowe, J. B. (2014). Beyond the “urge to move”: objective measures for the study of agency in the post-Libet era. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8(JUNE), 1–13. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00450.
Article
Google Scholar
Wolpert, D., Ghahramani, Z., & Jordan, M. (1995). An internal model for sensorimotor integration. Science, 269(5232), 1880–1882. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.7569931.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Yoshie, M., & Haggard, P. (2013). Negative emotional outcomes attenuate sense of agency over voluntary actions. Current Biology, 23(20), 2028–2032. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.08.034.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar