Skip to main content
Log in

Memory for item–location bindings is enhanced in older adults with appetitive motivationally laden pictures

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Both the selection and consumption of food are biologically necessary for survival. Consequently, individuals may consider food as a primary and biologically relevant stimulus. In addition, recent findings support specific patterns of food preference during the lifespan development. Indeed, the preference for sweet taste largely observed in newborns and children seems to decline in young adults and then re-emerge again in older adults. This motivational preference for sweet food in older adults may be potentially useful in the cognitive domain since many studies have found that motivationally or emotionally laden information is more likely to be detected, stored in memory and retrieved better than neutral information. To address this issue, we designed an item–location binding task with sweet food, savory food and object pictures, and asked young and older adults to maintain information in working memory and respond based on memory for either individual features or feature combination (i.e., identification, location, or combined identification + location information). Results evidenced a significant enhancement of older adults’ performance in the binding of motivationally relevant stimuli and their location, evidencing the potential usefulness of motivationally laden stimuli in promoting more effective binding processes and probably, more general working memory processes.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Allen, R. J., Brown, L. A., & Niven, E. (2013). Aging and visual feature binding in working memory. In H. St Clair-Thompson (Ed.), Working memory: Developmental differences, component processes, and improvement mechanisms (pp. 83–96). New York, NY: Nova Science.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bailey, R. L. (2015). Processing food advertisements: Initial biological responses matter. Communication Monographs, 82(1), 163–178.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bellisle, F., Rolland-Cachera, M. F., & Kellogg Scientific Advisory Committee ‘Child, and Nutrition. (2000). Three consecutive (1993, 1995, 1997) surveys of food intake, nutritional attitudes and knowledge, and lifestyle in 1000 French children, aged 9–11 years. Journal of Human Nutrition and Dietetics, 13(2), 101–111.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berridge, K. C. (2000). Measuring hedonic impact in animals and infants: Microstructure of affective taste reactivity patterns. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 24(2), 173–198.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Borella, E., Carretti, B., Zanoni, G., Zavagnin, M., & De Beni, R. (2013). Working memory training in old age: An examination of transfer and maintenance effects. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 28(4), 331–347.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, M. M., Greenwald, M. K., Petry, M. C., & Lang, P. J. (1992). Remembering pictures: pleasure and arousal in memory. Journal of experimental psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18(2), 379.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buchanan, T. W. (2007). Retrieval of emotional memories. Psychological Bulletin, 133(5), 761.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Carstensen, L. L., Fung, H. H., & Charles, S. T. (2003). Socioemotional selectivity theory and the regulation of emotion in the second half of life. Motivation and Emotion, 27(2), 103–123.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chalfonte, B. L., & Johnson, M. K. (1996). Feature memory and binding in young and older adults. Memory and Cognition, 24, 403–416.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Codispoti, M., Ferrari, V., & Bradley, M. M. (2007). Repetition and event-related potentials: distinguishing early and late processes in affective picture perception. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(4), 577–586.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. D., Perlstein, W. M., Braver, T. S., Nystrom, L. E., Noll, D. C., Jonides, J., et al. (1997). Temporal dynamics of brain activation during a working memory task. Nature, 386(6625), 604.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Craik, F. I., & Rose, N. S. (2012). Memory encoding and aging: a neurocognitive perspective. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(7), 1729–1739.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, W. A., & Brosch, T. (2012). Motivational salience: Amygdala tuning from traits, needs, values, and goals. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(1), 54–59.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dulas, M. R., & Duarte, A. (2013). The influence of directed attention at encoding on source memory retrieval in the young and old: An ERP study. Brain Research, 1500, 55–71.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Elliot, A. J., & McGregor, H. A. (2001). A 2 x 2 achievement goal framework. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 501–519.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fairfield, B., Mammarella, N., & Di Domenico, A. (2015). Motivated goal pursuit and working memory: Are there age-related differences? Motivation and Emotion, 39(2), 201–215.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fairfield, B., Mammarella, N., & Domenico, A. D. (2013). Centenarians’ “holy” memory: Is being positive enough? The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 174(1), 42–50.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12(3), 189–198.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Foroni, F., Pergola, G., Argiris, G., & Rumiati, R. I. (2013). The FoodCast research image database (FRIDa). Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 51.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gable, P. A., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2008). Approach-motivated positive affect reduces breadth of attention. Psychological Science, 19(5), 476–482.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gable, P., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2010). The motivational dimensional model of affect: Implications for breadth of attention, memory, and cognitive categorisation. Cognition and Emotion, 24(2), 322–337.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, T., Strunk, J., Arndt, J., & Duarte, A. (2016). Age-related deficits in selective attention during encoding increase demands on episodic reconstruction during context retrieval: An ERP study. Neuropsychologia, 86, 66–79.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, M. M. (1990). Age differences in decision making: A process methodology for examining strategic information processing. Journal of Gerontology, 45(2), P75–P78.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kennedy, Q., Mather, M., & Carstensen, L. L. (2004). The role of motivation in the age-related positivity effect in autobiographical memory. Psychological Science, 15, 208–214.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kensinger, E. A. (2004). Remembering emotional experiences: The contribution of valence and arousal. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 15(4), 241–252.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kensinger, E. A. (2009). Remembering the details: Effects of emotion. Emotion Review, 1(2), 99–113.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kuo, T. Y., & Van Petten, C. (2006). Prefrontal engagement during source memory retrieval depends on the prior encoding task. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(7), 1133–1146.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • LaBar, K. S., & Phelps, E. A. (1998). Arousal-mediated memory consolidation: Role of the medial temporal lobe in humans. Psychological Science, 9(6), 490–493.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larson, C. L., & Steuer, E. L. (2009). Motivational relevance as a potential modulator of memory for affective stimuli: Can we compare snakes and cakes? Emotion Review, 1(2), 116–117.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mather, M. (2007). Emotional arousal and memory binding: An object-based framework. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2(1), 33–52.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mather, M., & Carstensen, L. L. (2005). Aging and motivated cognition: The positivity effect in attention and memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(10), 496–502.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, K. J., Johnson, M. K., Raye, C. L., & D’Esposito, M. (2000). fMRI evidence of age-related hippocampal dysfunction in feature binding in working memory. Cognitive Brain Research, 10, 197–206.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Naveh-Benjamin, M., Brav, T. K., & Levy, O. (2007). The associative memory deficit of older adults: the role of strategy utilization. Psychology and Aging, 22(1), 202.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Padulo, C., Carlucci, L., Manippa, V., Marzoli, D., Saggino, A., Tommasi, L., et al. (2017). Valence, familiarity and arousal of different foods in relation to age, sex and weight. Food Quality and Preference, 57, 104–113.

    Google Scholar 

  • Padulo, C., Carlucci, L., Marzoli, D., Manippa, V., Tommasi, L., Saggino, A., et al. (2018). Affective evaluation of food images according to stimulus and subject characteristics. Journal of Human Nutrition and Dietetics, 31(6), 715–724.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Padulo, C., Pizzi, S. D., Bonanni, L., Edden, R. A., Ferretti, A., Marzoli, D., et al. (2016). GABA levels in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex during the viewing of appetitive and disgusting food images. Neuroscience, 333, 114–122.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Reisberg, D., & Heuer, F. (2004). Memory for Emotional events. In D. Reisberg & P. Hertel (Eds.), Series in affective science. Memory and emotion (pp. 3–41). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rozin, P. (2007). Food choice: an introduction. In L. Frewer & H. van Trijp (Eds.), Understanding consumers of food products (pp. 3–29). Cambridge: Woodhead Publishing Limited.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sharot, T., & Phelps, E. A. (2004). How arousal modulates memory: Disentangling the effects of attention and retention. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 4(3), 294–306.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. E., & Jonides, J. (1999). Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes. Science, 283(5408), 1657–1661.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, A., & Zhang, W. (2006). Location and binding in visual working memory. Memory & Cognition, 34(8), 1704–1719.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wheeler, M. E., & Treisman, A. M. (2002). Binding in short-term visual memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131(1), 48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yates, J. F., & Patalano, A. L. (1999). Decision making and aging. In D. C. Park & R. W. Morrell (Eds.), Processing of medical information in aging patients (pp. 31–54). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yonelinas, A. P. (2002). The nature of recollection and familiarity: A review of 30 years of research. Journal of Memory and Language, 46(3), 441–517.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Caterina Padulo.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Padulo, C., Mammarella, N., Brancucci, A. et al. Memory for item–location bindings is enhanced in older adults with appetitive motivationally laden pictures. Psychological Research 85, 951–957 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-020-01312-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-020-01312-5

Keywords

Navigation