Advertisement

Intelligence and working memory: evidence from administering the WAIS-IV to Italian adults and elderly

  • Erika BorellaEmail author
  • Lina Pezzuti
  • Rossana De Beni
  • Cesare Cornoldi
Original Article
  • 82 Downloads

Abstract

The present study took advantage of data collected on more than 2100 Italian adult and elderly individuals during the standardization of the WAIS-IV to examine the relationship between working memory (WM) components and intelligence, and how age affects this relationship. Administering the WAIS-IV enabled us to obtain five different measures for assessing different aspects of WM, and a measure of General Ability (GA) strongly loading on the g-factor. The main results were as follows: (1) age-related impairments in WM are substantial, and they are partly similar and partly different for the various WM measures; (2) the relationship between the WM measures and the General Ability Index (GAI) varies, becoming stronger when the active control required by the WM task is higher; (3) comparing the WM–GAI relationships between different age groups reveals some similar patterns, as well as some specific effects that depend on the WM task considered.

Notes

References

  1. Ackerman, P. L., Beier, M. E., & Boyle, M. O. (2005). Working memory and intelligence: The same or different constructs? Psychological Bulletin, 131, 30–60.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.131.1.30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Alloway, T. P., & Alloway, R. G. (2010). Investigating the predictive roles of working memory and IQ in academic attainment. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 106, 20–29.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2009.11.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Alloway, T. P., Gathercole, S. E., & Pickering, S. J. (2006). Verbal and visuospatial short-term memory in children: Are they separable? Child Development, 77, 1698–1716.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00968.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Baddeley, A. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press/Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
  5. Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 417–423.  https://doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01538-2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Balinsky, B. (1941). An analysis of the mental factors of various age groups from nine to sixty. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 23, 191–234.Google Scholar
  7. Baltes, P. B., & Lindenberger, U. (1997). Emergence of a powerful connection between sensory and cognitive functions across the adult life span: A new window to the study of cognitive aging? Psychology and Aging, 12, 12.  https://doi.org/10.1037//0882-7974.12.1.12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Baltes, P. B., Lindenberger, U., & Staudinger, U. M. (1998). Life span theory in developmental psychology. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (5th ed., pp. 1029–1143).  https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470147658.chpsy0111.Google Scholar
  9. Belacchi, C., Carretti, B., & Cornoldi, C. (2010). The role of working memory and updating in Coloured Raven Matrices performance in typically developing children. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 22, 1010–1020.  https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440903184617.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Bisiacchi, P., Borella, E., Bergamaschi, S., Carretti, B., & Mondini, S. (2008). Interplay between memory and executive functions in normal and pathological aging. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 30, 723–733.  https://doi.org/10.1080/13803390701689587.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Bopp, K. L., & Verhaeghen, P. (2005). Aging and verbal memory span: A meta-analysis. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 60B, 223–233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Borella, E., Carretti, B., & De Beni, R. (2008). Working memory and inhibition across the adult life-span. Acta Psychologica, 128, 33–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Borella, E., Delaloye, C., Lecerf, T., Renaud, O., & de Ribaupierre, A. (2009). Do age differences between young and older adults in inhibitory tasks depend on the degree of activation of information? European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 21, 445–472.  https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440802613997.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Carretti, B., Belacchi, C., & Cornoldi, C. (2010). Difficulties in working memory updating in individuals with intellectual disability. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 54, 337–345.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2788.2010.01267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Case, R. (1985). Intellectual development: A systematic reinterpretation. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  16. Case, R. (1987). The structure and process of intellectual development. International Journal of Psychology, 22, 571–607.  https://doi.org/10.1080/00207598708246796.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Colom, R., Abad, F. J., Quiroga, M. Á., Shih, P. C., & Flores-Mendoza, C. (2008). Working memory and intelligence are highly related constructs, but why? Intelligence, 36, 584–606.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2008.01.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Conway, A. R. A., Getz, S. J., Macnamara, B., & Engel de Abreu, P. M. J. (2011). Working memory and fluid intelligence: A multi-mechanism view. In R. J. Sternberg & S. B. Kaufman (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of intelligence. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  19. Cornoldi, C., & Giofrè, D. (2014). The crucial role of working memory in intellectual functioning. European Psychologist, 19, 260–268.  https://doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040/a000183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Cornoldi, C., & Oakhill, J. (1996). Reading comprehension difficulties. Mahwah: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  21. Cornoldi, C., Orsini, A., Cianci, L., Giofrè, D., & Pezzuti, L. (2013). Intelligence and working memory control: Evidence from the WISC-IV administration to Italian children. Learning and Individual Differences, 26, 9–14.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2013.04.005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Cornoldi, C., & Vecchi, T. (2003). Visuo-spatial working memory and individual differences. Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  23. Craik, F. I. M., & Salthouse, T. A. (2008). Handbook of cognitive aging (3rd ed.). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  24. de Ribaupierre, A. (2001). Working memory and attentional processes across the lifespan. In P. Graf & N. Otha (Eds.), Lifespan development of human memory (pp. 59–80). Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  25. de Ribaupierre, A., & Lecerf, T. (2006). Relationships between working memory and intelligence: Convergent evidence from a Neo-Piagetian and a psychometric approach. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 18, 109–137.  https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440500216127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Egeland, J. (2015). Measuring working memory with digit span and the letter-number sequencing subtests from the WAIS-IV: Too low manipulation load and risk for underestimating modality effects. Applied Neuropsychology: Adult, 22(6), s445–s451.  https://doi.org/10.1080/23279095.2014.992069. (ISSN:2327-9095).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Engle, R. W. (2002). Working memory capacity as executive attention. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 19–23.  https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.00160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Flanagan, D. P., & Kaufman, S. (2004). Essentials of assessment with WISC-IV. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  29. Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189–198.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Ghisletta, P., & Lindenberger, U. (2003). Age-based structural dynamics between perceptual speed and knowledge in the Berlin Aging Study: Direct evidence for ability dedifferentiation in old age. Psychology and Aging, 18, 696–713.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.18.4.696.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Giofrè, D., & Cornoldi, C. (2015). The structure of intelligence in children with specific learning disabilities is different as compared to typically development children. Intelligence, 52, 36–43.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2015.07.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Giofrè, D., Ferioli, P., Stoppa, E., Pezzuti, L., & Cornoldi, C. (2016). Differences in forward and backward digit-span in children with normal development and with specific learning disabilities. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 38, 478–486.  https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2015.1125454.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Giofrè, D., Mammarella, I. C., & Cornoldi, C. (2013). The structure of working memory and how it relates to intelligence in children. Intelligence, 41, 396–406.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2013.06.006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Hofer, S. M., & Sliwinski, M. J. (2001). Understanding ageing: An evaluation of research designs for assessing the interdependence of ageing-related changes. Gerontology, 47, 341–352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Jumel, B. (2008). Guide clinique des tests chez l’enfant. Paris: Dunod.Google Scholar
  36. Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1992). A capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences in working memory. Psychological Review, 99, 122–149.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.99.1.122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Kane, M. J., Hambrick, D. Z., & Conway, A. R. A. (2005). Working memory capacity and fluid intelligence are strongly related constructs: Comment on Ackerman, Beier, and Boyle (2005). Psychological Bulletin, 131, 66–71.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.131.1.66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Kane, M. J., Hambrick, D. Z., Tuholski, S. W., Wilhelm, O., Payne, T. W., & Engle, R. W. (2004). The generality of working memory capacity: A latent-variable approach to verbal and visuospatial memory span and reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 189–217.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.133.2.189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Kaufmann, A. S. (1994). Intelligence testing with the WISC-III (p. 70). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  40. Kyllonen, C., & Christal, R. E. (1990). Reasoning ability is (little more than) working memory capacity? Intelligence, 14, 389–433.  https://doi.org/10.1016/s0160-2896(05)80012-1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Lanfranchi, S., Cornoldi, C., Drigo, S., & Vianello, R. (2008). Working memory in individuals with fragile X syndrome. Child Neuropsychology, 15, 105–119.  https://doi.org/10.1080/09297040802112564.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Lanfranchi, S., Cornoldi, C., & Vianello, R. (2004). Verbal and visuospatial working memory deficits in children with Down syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 6, 456–466.  https://doi.org/10.1352/0895-8017(2004)109%3c456:vavwmd%3e2.0.co;2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Lecerf, T., Reverte, I., Coleaux, L., Favez, N., & Rossier, J. (2010). Indice d’aptitude général pour le WISC-IV: Normes francophones. Pratiques Psychologiques, 16, 109–121.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.prps.2009.04.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Li, S. C., Lindenberger, U., Hommel, B., Aschersleben, G., Prinz, W., & Baltes, P. B. (2004). Lifespan transformations in the couplings of mental abilities and underlying cognitive processes. Psychological Science, 15, 155–163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Li, S. C., & Schmiedek, F. (2002). Age is not necessarily aging: Another step towards understanding the ‘clocks’ that time aging. Gerontology, 48, 5–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Lindenberger, U., & Baltes, P. B. (1997). Intellectual functioning in old and very old age: Cross-sectional results from the Berlin Aging Study. Psychology and Aging, 12(3), 410.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Mammarella, I. C., Borella, E., Pastore, M., & Pazzaglia, F. (2013). The structure of visuo-spatial abilities in adulthood. Learning and Individual Differences, 25, 99–110.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219411400746.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Marzocchi, G. M., Lucangeli, D., De Meo, T., Fini, F., & Cornoldi, C. (2002). The disturbing effect of irrelevant information on arithmetic problem solving in inattentive children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 21, 73–92.  https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326942dn2101_4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Miller, L. J., Myers, A., Prinzi, L., & Mittenberg, W. (2009). Changes in intellectual functioning associated with normal aging. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 24, 681–688.  https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acp072.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Miyake, A., & Shah, P. (1999). Toward unified theories of working memory: Emerging general consensus, unresolved theoretical issues, and future research directions. In A. Miyake & P. Shah (Eds.), Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control (pp. 442–481). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Myerson, J., Emery, L., White, D. A., & Hale, S. (2003). Effects of age, domain, and processing demands on memory span: Evidence for differential decline. Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition, 10, 20–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Oberauer, K., Schulze, R., Wilhelm, O., & Süss, H.-M. (2005). Working memory and intelligence—their correlation and their relation: Comment on Ackerman, Beier, and Boyle (2005). Psychological Bulletin, 131, 61–65.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.131.1.61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Orsini, A., & Pezzuti, L. (2013). WAIS-IV. Contributo alla taratura italiana (16–69) (WAIS-IV, Contribution to the Italian Standardization, ages 16–69). Firenze: Giunti OS.Google Scholar
  54. Orsini, A., & Pezzuti, L. (2014). L’indice di abilità generale della scala WISC-IV (The general ability index in the WISC-IV). Psicologia Clinica dello Sviluppo, XVIII(2), 301–310.Google Scholar
  55. Orsini, A., & Pezzuti, L. (2015). WAIS-IV. Contributo alla taratura italiana (70–90 anni). (WAIS-IV, contribution to the Italian standardization, ages 16–69). Firenze: Giunti OS.Google Scholar
  56. Orsini, A., Pezzuti, L., & Picone, L. (2011). WISC-IV. Contributo alla taratura Italiana. [WISC-IV Italian Edition]. Firenze: Giunti OS.Google Scholar
  57. Park, D. C., Lautenschlager, G., Hedden, T., Davidson, N. S., Smith, A. D., & Smith, P. K. (2002). Models of visuospatial and verbal memory across the adult life span. Psychology and Aging, 17, 299–320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Pezzuti, L., & Rossetti, S. (2017). Letter-number sequencing, figure weights, and cancellation subtests of WAIS-IV administered to elders. Personality and Individual Differences, 104, 352–356.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.08.019.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Pezzuti, L., Lauriola, M., Borella, E., De Beni, R., & Cornoldi, C. (2019). Working Memory and Processing Speed mediate the effect of age on a General Ability Construct: Evidence from the Italian WAIS-IV standardization sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 138(1), 298–304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Raiford, S. E., Weiss, P. D. L. G., Rolfhus, P. D. E., & Coalson, P. D. D. (2005). General Ability Index. Hartcourt Assessment, Technical Report.Google Scholar
  61. Redick, T. S., Unsworth, N., Kelly, A. J., & Engle, R. W. (2012). Faster, smarter? Working memory capacity and perceptual speed in relation to fluid intelligence. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 24, 844–854.  https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2012.704359.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., Jonides, J., Smith, E., Hartley, A., Miller, A., Marshuetz, C., & Koeppe, R. (2000). Age differences in the frontal lateralization of verbal and spatial working memory revealed by PET. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 174–187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Robert, C., Borella, E., Fagot, D., Lecerf, T., & de Ribaupierre, A. (2009). Working memory and inhibitory control across the life span: Intrusion errors in the Reading Span Task. Memory & Cognition, 37(3), 336–345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Saklofske, D. H., Prifitera, A., Weiss, L. G., Rolfhus, E., & Zhu, J. (2005). Clinical interpretation of the WISC-IV FSIQ and GAI. In A. Prifitera, D. H. Saklofske, & L. G. Weiss (Eds.), WISC-IV. Clinical use and interpretation. San Diego: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  65. Salthouse, T. A., & Saklofske, D. H. (2010). Do the WAIS-IV tests measure the same aspects of cognitive functioning in adults under and over age 65? In L. G. Weiss, D. H. Saklofske, D. L. Coalson, & S. E. Raiford (Eds.), WAIS-IV clinical use and interpretation (pp. 217–235). San Diego: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. San Miguel Montes, L. E., Allen, D. N., Puente, A. E., & Neblina, C. (2010). Validity of the WISC-IV Spanish for a clinically referred sample of Hispanic children. Psychological Assessment, 22, 465–469.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018895.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Schaie, K. W., & Willis, S. L. (2010). The Seattle Longitudinal Study of adult cognitive development. ISSBD Bulletin, 57, 24–29.Google Scholar
  68. Smith, E. E., & Jonides, J. (1999). Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes. Science, 283(5408), 1657–1661.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Swanson, H. L., & Siegel, L. S. (2001). Learning disabilities as a working memory deficit. Issues in Education, 7, 1–48.Google Scholar
  70. Unsworth, N., Brewer, G. A., & Spillers, G. J. (2009). There’s more to the working memory capacity-fluid intelligence relationship than just secondary memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 931–937.  https://doi.org/10.3758/pbr.16.5.931.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Vecchi, T., & Cornoldi, C. (1999). Passive storage and active manipulation in visuo-spatial working memory: Further evidence from the study of age differences. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 11, 391–406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Wechsler, D. (2003). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for children-fourth edition. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
  73. Wechsler, D. (2008). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale fourth edition: Technical and interpretive manual. San Antonio: Pearson.Google Scholar
  74. Wisdom, N. M., Mignogna, J., & Collins, R. L. (2012). Variability in Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-IV subtest performance across age. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 27, 389–397.  https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acs041.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  • Erika Borella
    • 1
    Email author
  • Lina Pezzuti
    • 2
  • Rossana De Beni
    • 1
  • Cesare Cornoldi
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of General PsychologyUniversity of PadovaPaduaItaly
  2. 2.Sapienza University of RomeRomeItaly

Personalised recommendations