Abstract
Our world is a sonically busy place and we use both acoustic information and experience-based knowledge to make sense of the sounds arriving at our ears. The knowledge we gain through experience has the potential to shape what sounds are prioritized in a complex scene. There are many examples of how visual expertise influences how we perceive objects in visual scenes, but few studies examine how auditory expertise is associated with attentional biases toward familiar real-world sounds in complex scenes. In the current study, we investigated whether musical expertise is associated with the ability to detect changes to real-world sounds in complex auditory scenes, and whether any such benefit is specific to musical instrument sounds. We also examined whether change detection is better for human-generated sounds in general or only communicative human sounds. We found that musicians had less change deafness overall. All listeners were better at detecting human communicative sounds compared to human non-communicative sounds, but this benefit was driven by speech sounds and sounds that were vocally generated. Musical listening skill, speech-in-noise, and executive function abilities were used to predict rates of change deafness. Auditory memory, musical training, fine-grained pitch processing, and an interaction between training and pitch processing accounted for 45.8% of the variance in change deafness. To better understand perceptual and cognitive expertise, it may be more important to measure various auditory skills and relate them to each other, as opposed to comparing experts to non-experts.
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.






Notes
It should be noted that because a change during a trial inherently involves two different sounds (i.e., dropping from scene 1 and replacing in scene 2, see Fig. 1), two different superordinate categories are involved in every “different” trial. All analyses were done separately when superordinate categories were grouped by the sound dropping out of scene 1 and when grouped by the sound replacing in scene 2 and results were the same. Given that participants have better memory for the changing sound from scene 2 than scene 1 and that encoding of the changing sound from scene 2 explains more variance in change detection performance than encoding the changing sound from scene 1 (Irsik, Vanden Bosch der Nederlanden, & Snyder, 2016), we decided to report results grouped by the replacing sound from scene 2. The superordinate category effect size for these data was larger for scene 2 (η 2p = 0.300) than scene 1 (η 2p = 0.137), consistent with our previous work.
References
Agres, K., & Krumhansl, C. L. (2008). Musical change deafness: The inability to detect change in a non-speech auditory domain. Paper presented at the 30th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Washington, DC.
Alain, C., & Arnott, S. R. (2000). Selectively attending to auditory objects. Frontiers in Bioscience,5, D202–D212.
Bialystok, E., & DePape, A. M. (2009). Musical expertise, bilingualism, and executive functioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,35(2), 565. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0012735.
Bidelman, G. M., Hutka, S., & Moreno, S. (2013). Tone language speakers and musicians share enhanced perceptual and cognitive abilities for musical pitch: evidence for bidirectionality between the domains of language and music. PLoS One,8(4), e60676. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0060676.
Bilger, R. C., Nuetzel, J. M., Rabinowitz, W. M., & Rzeczkowski, C. (1984). Standardization of a test of speech perception in noise. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research,27(1), 32. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.2701.32.
Bishop, L., & Goebl, W. (2014). Context-specific effects of musical expertise on audiovisual integration. Frontiers in Psychology. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01123.
Boebinger, D., Evans, S., Scott, S. K., Rosen, S., Lima, C. F., & Manly, T. (2015). Musicians and non-musicians are equally adept at perceiving masked speech. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America,137(1), 378–387. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.4904537.
Bregman, A. S. (1990). Auditory scene analysis (Vol. 10). Cambridge: MIT press.
Carey, D., Rosen, S., Krishnan, S., Pearce, M. T., Shepherd, A., Aydelott, J., & Dick, F. (2015). Generality and specificity in the effects of musical expertise on perception and cognition. Cognition,137, 81–105. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.005.
Chartrand, J. P., Filion-Bilodeau, S., & Belin, P. (2007). Brain response to birdsongs in bird experts. NeuroReport,18(4), 335–340. https://doi.org/10.1097/WNR.0b013e328013cea9.
Chartrand, J. P., Peretz, I., & Belin, P. (2008). Auditory recognition expertise and domain specificity. Brain Research,1220, 191–198. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2008.01.014.
Cohen, M. A., Evans, K. K., Horowitz, T. S., & Wolfe, J. M. (2011). Auditory and visual memory in musicians and nonmusicians. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,18(3), 586–591. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-011-0074-0.
Corrigall, K. A., Schellenberg, E. G., & Misura, N. M. (2013). Music training, cognition, and personality. Frontiers in Psychology,4, 222. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00222.
Corrigall, K. A., & Trainor, L. J. (2011). Associations between length of music training and reading skills in children. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal,29(2), 147–155. https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2011.29.2.147.
Cousineau, D. (2005). Confidence intervals in within-subject designs: A simpler solution to Loftus and Masson’s method. Tutorials in quantitative methods for psychology,1(1), 42–45.
Darlington, R. B. (1990). Regression and linear models. New York: McGraw-Hill.
De Meo, R., Matusz, P. J., Knebel, J. F., Murray, M. M., Thompson, W. R., & Clarke, S. (2016). What makes medical students better listeners? Current Biology,26(13), R519–R520. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2016.05.024.
Demany, L., Trost, W., Serman, M., & Semal, C. (2008). Auditory change detection: Simple sounds are not memorized better than complex. Psychological Science,19(1), 85–91.
Dickerson, K., & Gaston, J. R. (2014). Did you hear that? The role of stimulus similarity and uncertainty in auditory change deafness. Frontiers in Psychology. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01125.
Ding, N., & Simon, J. Z. (2012). Emergence of neural encoding of auditory objects while listening to competing speakers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,109(29), 11854–11859. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1002793.
Eramudugolla, R., Irvine, D. R. F., McAnally, K. I., Martin, R. L., & Mattingley, J. B. (2005). Directed attention eliminates ‘change deafness’ in complex auditory scenes. Current Biology,15, 1108–1113.
Fenn, K. M., Shintel, H., Atkins, A. S., Skipper, J. I., Bond, V. C., & Nusbaum, H. C. (2011). When less is heard than meets the ear: Change deafness in a telephone conversation. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,64(7), 1442–1456. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2011.570353.
Flaugnacco, E., Lopez, L., Terribili, C., Montico, M., Zoia, S., & Schön, D. (2015). Music training increases phonological awareness and reading skills in developmental dyslexia: a randomized control trial. PLoS ONE,10(9), e0138715. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0138715.
Forgeard, M., Winner, E., Norton, A., & Schlaug, G. (2008). Practicing a musical instrument in childhood is associated with enhanced verbal ability and nonverbal reasoning. PLoS ONE,3(10), e3566. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0003566.
Gaser, C., & Schlaug, G. (2003a). Brain structures differ between musicians and non-musicians. Journal of Neuroscience,23(27), 9240–9245.
Gaser, C., & Schlaug, G. (2003b). Gray matter differences between musicians and nonmusicians. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences,999(1), 514–517. https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1284.062.
Grahn, J. A., & Rowe, J. B. (2009). Feeling the beat: premotor and striatal interactions in musicians and nonmusicians during beat perception. Journal of Neuroscience,29(23), 7540–7548. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2018-08.2009.
Gregg, M. K., Irsik, V. C., & Snyder, J. S. (2014). Change deafness and object encoding with recognizable and unrecognizable sounds. Neuropsychologia,61, 19–30.
Gregg, M. K., Irsik, V. C., & Snyder, J. S. (2017). Effects of capacity limits, memory loss, and sound type in change deafness. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics,79(8), 1–12.
Gregg, M. K., & Samuel, A. G. (2008). Change deafness and the organizational properties of sounds. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,34(4), 974. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.34.4.974.
Gregg, M. K., & Samuel, A. G. (2009). The importance of semantics in auditory representations. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics,71(3), 607–619. https://doi.org/10.3758/APP.71.3.607.
Hayes, A. F. (2015). An index and test of linear moderated mediation. Multivariate Behavioral Research,50, 1–22.
Irsik, V. C., Vanden Bosch der Nederlanden, C. M., & Snyder, J. S. (2016). Broad attention to multiple individual objects may facilitate change detection with complex auditory scenes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,42(11), 1806–1817.
Jakobson, L. S., Cuddy, L. L., & Kilgour, A. R. (2003). Time tagging: A key to musicians’ superior memory. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal,20(3), 307–313. https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2003.20.3.307.
Jones, B. T., Bruce, G., Livingstone, S., & Reed, E. (2006). Alcohol-related attentional bias in problem drinkers with the flicker change blindness paradigm. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors,20(2), 171.
Kikichi, Y., Senju, A., Tojo, Y., Osanai, H., & Hasegawa, T. (2009). Faces do not capture special attention in children with autism spectrum disorder: A change blindness study. Child Development,80(5), 1421–1433.
Krishnan, S., Leech, R., Aydelott, J., & Dick, F. (2013). School-age children’s environmental object identification in natural auditory scenes: Effects of masking and contextual congruence. Hearing Research,300, 46–55. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.heares.2013.03.003.
LaPointe, M. R., Cullen, R., Baltaretu, B., Campos, M., Michalski, N., Sri Satgunarajah, S., … Shore, D. I. (2016). An attentional bias for LEGO® people using a change detection task: Are LEGO® people animate? Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale,70(3), 219. https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000077.
Law, L. N., & Zentner, M. (2012). Assessing musical abilities objectively: construction and validation of the profile of music perception skills. PLoS ONE,7(12), e52508. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0052508.
Loui, P., Kroog, K., Zuk, J., Winner, E., & Schlaug, G. (2011). Relating pitch awareness to phonemic awareness in children: implications for tone-deafness and dyslexia. Frontiers in Psychology,2, 111. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00111.
Madsen, S. M. K., Whiteford, K. L., & Oxenham, A. J. (2017). Musicians do not benefit from differences in fundamental frequency when listening to speech in competing speech backgrounds. Scientific Reports,7(12624), 1–9.
Magne, C., Schön, D., & Besson, M. (2006). Musician children detect pitch violations in both music and language better than nonmusician children: behavioral and electrophysiological approaches. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,18(2), 199–211. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2006.18.2.199.
Marie, C., Fujioka, T., Herrington, L., & Trainor, L. J. (2012). The high-voice superiority effect in polyphonic music is influenced by experience: A comparison of musicians who play soprano-range compared with bass-range instruments. Psychomusicology, Music, Mind and Brain,22(2), 97. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030858.
Micheyl, C., Delhommeau, K., Perrot, X., & Oxenham, A. J. (2006). Influence of musical and psychoacoustical training on pitch discrimination. Hearing Research,219(1), 36–47. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.heares.2006.05.004.
Neuhoff, J. G., & Bochtler, K. S. (2018). Change deafness, dual-task performance, and domain-specific expertise. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71(5), 1100–1111.
Neuhoff, J. G., Schott, S. A., Kropf, A. J., & Neuhoff, E. M. (2014). Familiarity, expertise, and change detection: Change deafness is worse in your native language. Perception, 43, 219–222.
New, J., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2007). Category-specific attention for animals reflects ancestral priorities, not expertise. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,104(42), 16598–16603. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0703913104.
Oxenham, A. J., Fligor, B. J., Mason, C. R., & Kidd, G., Jr. (2003). Informational masking and musical training. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America,114(3), 1543–1549. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.1598197.
Pantev, C., Roberts, L. E., Schulz, M., Engelien, A., & Ross, B. (2001). Timbre-specific enhancement of auditory cortical representations in musicians. NeuroReport,12(1), 169–174.
Parbery-Clark, A., Skoe, E., Lam, C., & Kraus, N. (2009). Musician enhancement for speech-in-noise. Ear and Hearing,30(6), 653–661. https://doi.org/10.1097/AUD.0b013e3181b412e9.
Pasinski, A. C., Hannon, E. E., & Snyder, J. S. (2016). How musical are music video game players? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,23(5), 1553–1558.
Ruggles, D. R., Freyman, R. L., & Oxenham, A. J. (2014). Influence of musical training on understanding voiced and whispered speech in noise. PLoS ONE,9(1), e86980. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0086980.
Schellenberg, E. G. (2011). Examining the association between music lessons and intelligence. British Journal of Psychology,102(3), 283–302. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.2010.02000.x.
Schellenberg, E. G. (2015). Music training and speech perception: a gene–environment interaction. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences,1337(1), 170–177. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.12627.
Schön, D., Magne, C., & Besson, M. (2004). The music of speech: Music training facilitates pitch processing in both music and language. Psychophysiology,41(3), 341–349. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-8986.00172.x.
Shahin, A., Roberts, L. E., & Trainor, L. J. (2004). Enhancement of auditory cortical development by musical experience in children. NeuroReport,15(12), 1917–1921.
Shen, J., Mack, M. L., & Palmeri, T. J. (2014). Studying real-world perceptual expertise. Frontiers in Psychology. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00857.
Sherwin, J. S., & Gaston, J. R. (2015). Experience does not equal expertise in recognizing infrequent incoming gunfire: neural markers for experience and task expertise at peak behavioral performance. PLoS One,10(2), e0115629. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0115629.
Shinn-Cunningham, B. G. (2008). Object-based auditory and visual attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,12(5), 182–186. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.02.003.
Shultz, S., & Vouloumanos, A. (2010). Three-month-olds prefer speech to other naturally occurring signals. Language Learning and Development,6(4), 241–257. https://doi.org/10.1080/15475440903507830.
Shultz, S., Vouloumanos, A., Bennett, R. H., & Pelphrey, K. (2014). Neural specialization for speech in the first months of life. Developmental Science,17(5), 766–774. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12151.
Simons, D. J. (2000). Current approaches to Change Blindness. Visual Cognition,7(1–3), 1–15.
Slevc, L. R., Davey, N. S., Buschkuehl, M., & Jaeggi, S. M. (2016). Tuning the mind: Exploring the connections between musical ability and executive functions. Cognition,152, 199–211. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.03.017.
Snyder, J. S., & Elhilali, M. (2017). Recent advances in exploring the neural underpinnings of auditory scene perception. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences,1396(1), 39–55.
Snyder, J. S., & Gregg, M. K. (2011). Memory for sound, with an ear toward hearing in complex auditory scenes. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics,73(7), 1993. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-011-0189-4.
Snyder, J. S., Gregg, M. K., Weintraub, D. M., & Alain, C. (2012). Attention, awareness, and the perception of auditory scenes. Frontiers in Psychology. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00015.
Swaminathan, J., Mason, C. R., Streeter, T. M., Best, V., Kidd, G., Jr., & Patel, A. D. (2015). Musical training, individual differences and the cocktail party problem. Scientific Reports,5, 11628. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep11628.
Swaminathan, S., & Schellenberg, E. G. (2018). Musical competence is predicted by music training, cognitive abilities, and personality. Scientific Reports,8(1), 9223.
Swaminathan, S., Schellenberg, E.G., & Venkatesan, K. (2018). Explaining the association between music training and reading in adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 44(6), 992–999.
Vanden Bosch der Nederlanden, C. M., Snyder, J. S., & Hannon, E. E. (2016). Children use object-level category knowledge to detect changes in complex auditory scenes. Developmental Psychology,52(11), 1867. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000211.
Weiss, M. W., Schellenberg, E. G., Trehub, S. E., & Dawber, E. J. (2015a). Enhanced processing of vocal melodies in childhood. Developmental Psychology,51(3), 370. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038784.
Weiss, M. W., Trehub, S. E., & Schellenberg, E. G. (2012). Something in the way she sings: Enhanced memory for vocal melodies. Psychological Science,23(10), 1074–1078. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612442552.
Weiss, M. W., Vanzella, P., Schellenberg, E. G., & Trehub, S. E. (2015b). Pianists exhibit enhanced memory for vocal melodies but not piano melodies. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,68(5), 866–877. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2015.1020818.
Werner, S., & Thies, B. (2000). Is “change blindness” attenuated by domain-specific expertise? An expert-novices comparison of change detection in football images. Visual Cognition,7(1–3), 163–173. https://doi.org/10.1080/135062800394748.
Whisman, M. A., & McClelland, G. H. (2005). Designing, testing, and interpreting interactions and moderator effects in family research. Journal of Family Psychology,19(1), 111–120.
Wilson, R. H., McArdle, R., Watts, K. L., & Smith, S. L. (2012). The Revised SpeechPerception in Noise Test (R-SPIN) in a multiple signal-to-noise ratio paradigm. Journal of the American Academy of Audiology,23(8), 590–605. https://doi.org/10.3766/jaaa.23.7.9.
Zendel, B. R., & Alain, C. (2009). Concurrent sound segregation is enhanced in musicians. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,21(8), 1488–1498. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21140.
Zendel, B. R., & Alain, C. (2012). Musicians experience less age-related decline in central auditory processing. Psychology and Aging,27(2), 410. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024816.
Zendel, B. R., & Alain, C. (2014). Enhanced attention-dependent activity in the auditory cortex of older musicians. Neurobiology of Aging,35(1), 55–63. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2013.06.022.
Zentner, M., & Strauss, H. (2017). Assessing musical ability quickly and objectively: development and validation of the Short-PROMS and the Mini-PROMS. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences,1400(1), 33–45. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.13410.
Funding
This research is supported by a grant awarded to J.S.S. from the Army Research Office, award number W911NF-12-1-0256 and the Barrick Graduate Fellowship awarded to C.M.V.B.d.N from the University of Nevada Las Vegas.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Conflict of interest
All authors declare no conflict of interest.
Ethical approval
All procedures performed in the current study involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional review board (IRB) at the University of Nevada, Las Vegas.
Informed consent
Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants during the study.
Electronic supplementary material
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Vanden Bosch der Nederlanden, C.M., Zaragoza, C., Rubio-Garcia, A. et al. Change detection in complex auditory scenes is predicted by auditory memory, pitch perception, and years of musical training. Psychological Research 84, 585–601 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1072-x
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1072-x