Skip to main content
Log in

How to point and to interpret pointing gestures? Instructions can reduce pointer–observer misunderstandings

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In everyday communication, people often point. However, a pointing act is often misinterpreted as indicating a different spatial referent position than intended by the pointer. It has been suggested that this happens because pointers put the tip of the index finger close to the line joining the eye to the referent. However, the person interpreting the pointing act extrapolates the vector defined by the arm and index finger. As this line crosses the eye-referent line, it suggests a different referent position than the one that was meant. In this paper, we test this hypothesis by manipulating the geometry underlying the production and interpretation of pointing gestures. In Experiment 1, we compared naïve pointer-observed dyads with dyads in which the discrepancy between the vectors defining the production and interpretation of pointing acts has been reduced. As predicted, this reduced pointer–observer misunderstandings compared to the naïve control group. In Experiment 2, we tested whether pointers elevate their arms steeper than necessary to orient it toward the referent, because they visually steer their index finger tips onto the referents in their visual field. Misunderstandings between pointers and observers were smaller when pointers pointed without visual feedback. In sum, the results support the hypothesis that misunderstandings between (naïve) pointers and observers result from different spatial rules describing the production and interpretation of pointing gestures. Furthermore, we suggest that instructions that reduce the discrepancy between these spatial rules can improve communicating with pointing gestures.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. We report Greenhouse Geisser corrected p values but uncorrected dfs throughout the article.

  2. One observer of a naive dyad and one observer of a head-on-shoulder dyad claimed to base referent estimates on the eye-finger line. The respective naïve dyad performed actually worse than the average naïve dyads. The errors of the respective head-on-shoulder dyad were about half the size of the errors of the remaining head-on-shoulder dyads.

References

  • Alibali, M. W. (2005). Gesture in spatial cognition: expressing, communicating, and thinking about spatial information. Spatial Cognition and Computation, 5(4), 307–331. doi:10.1207/s15427633scc0504_2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alibali, M. W., Kita, S., & Young, A. J. (2000). Gesture and the process of speech production: we think, therefore we gesture. Language and Cognitive Processes, 15(6), 593–613. doi:10.1080/016909600750040571.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alibali, M. W., Spencer, R. C., Knox, L., & Kita, S. (2011). Spontaneous gestures influence strategy choices in problem solving. Psychological Science, 22(9), 1138–1144. doi:10.1177/0956797611417722.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Armstrong, L., & Marks, L. E. (1997). Differential effects of stimulus context on perceived length: implications for the horizontal-vertical illusion. Perception and Psychophysics, 59(8), 1200–1213. doi:10.3758/BF03214208.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bangerter, A. (2004). Using pointing and describing to achieve joint focus of attention in dialogue. Psychological Science, 15(6), 415–419. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00694.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bangerter, A., & Oppenheimer, D. M. (2006). Accuracy in detecting referents of pointing gestures unaccompanied by language. Gesture, 6(1), 85–102. doi:10.1075/gest.6.1.05ban.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Behne, T., Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Twelve-month-olds’ comprehension and production of pointing. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30(3), 359–375. doi:10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02043.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Biederman, I., & Shiffrar, M. M. (1987). Sexing day-old chicks: A case study and expert systems analysis of a difficult perceptual-learning task. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13(4), 640–645. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.13.4.640.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Butterworth, G. (2003). Pointing is the royal road to language for babies. In S. Kita (Ed.), Pointing: Where language, culture, and cognition meet (pp. 9–33). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butterworth, G., & Itakura, S. (2000). How the eyes, head and hand serve definite reference. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 18(1), 25–50. doi:10.1348/026151000165553.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., Tomasello, M., Butterworth, G. & Moore, C. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 63(4), i–vi, 1–143. doi:10.2307/1166214.

  • Cleret de Langavant, L., Remy, P., Trinkler, I., McIntyre, J., Dupoux, E., Berthoz, A., & Bachoud-Lévi, A.-C. (2011). Behavioral and neural correlates of communication via pointing. PLoS ONE, 6(3), e17719. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0017719.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Coren, S. (1993). The lateral preference inventory for measurement of handedness, footedness, eyedness, and earedness: norms for young adults. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 31(1), 1–3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elliott, D., Dutoy, C., Andrew, M., Burkitt, J. J., Grierson, L. E., Lyons, J. L., … Bennett, S. J. (2014). The influence of visual feedback and prior knowledge about feedback on vertical aiming strategies. Journal of Motor Behavior, 46(6), 433–443. doi:10.1080/00222895.2014.933767.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Herbort, O., & Kunde, W. (2016). Spatial (mis-)interpretation of pointing gestures to distal referents. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42(1), 78–89. doi:10.1037/xhp0000126.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hermsdörfer, J., Li, Y., Randerath, J., Goldenberg, G., & Johannsen, L. (2012). Tool use without a tool: kinematic characteristics of pantomiming as compared to actual use and the effect of brain damage. Experimental Brain Research, 218(2), 201–214. doi:10.1007/s00221-012-3021-z.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hollingworth, H. L. (1910). The central tendency of judgment. The Journal of Philosophy, Psychology and Scientific Methods, 7(17), 461–469. doi:10.2307/2012819.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leavens, D. A., & Hopkins, W. D. (1999). The whole-hand point: the structure and function of pointing from a comparative perspective. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 113(4), 417–425. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.113.4.417.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lücking, A., Pfeiffer, T., & Rieser, H. (2015). Pointing and reference reconsidered. Journal of Pragmatics, 77, 56–79. doi:10.1016/j.pragma.2014.12.013.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mazzoni, P., & Krakauer, J. W. (2006). An implicit plan overrides an explicit strategy during visuomotor adaptation. The Journal of Neuroscience, 26(14), 3642–3645. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5317-05.2006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pechmann, T., & Deutsch, W. (1982). The development of verbal and nonverbal devices for reference. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 34(2), 330–341. doi:10.1016/0022-0965(82)90050-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peeters, D., Chu, M., Holler, J., Hagoort, P., & Özyürek, A. (2015). Electrophysiological and kinematic correlates of communicative intent in the planning and production of pointing gestures and speech. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27(12), 2352–2368. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_00865.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Poulter, D., Jackson, R., Wann, J., & Berry, D. (2005). The effect of learning condition on perceptual anticipation, awareness, and visual search. Human Movement Science, 24(3), 345–361. doi:10.1016/j.humov.2005.06.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rauscher, F. H., Krauss, R. M., & Chen, Y. (1996). Gesture, speech, and lexical access: The role of lexical movements in speech production. Psychological Science, 7(4), 226–231. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1996.tb00364.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roth, W.-M. (2001). Gestures: their role in teaching and learning. Review of Educational Research, 71(3), 365–392. doi:10.3102/00346543071003365.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sanchez, D. J., & Reber, P. J. (2013). Explicit pre-training instruction does not improve implicit perceptual-motor sequence learning. Cognition, 126(3), 341–351. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2012.11.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, J. L., & McCloskey, D. (1988). Pointing. Behavioural Brain Research, 29(1–2), 1–5. doi:10.1016/0166-4328(88)90046-0.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wnuczko, M., & Kennedy, J. M. (2011). Pivots for pointing: visually-monitored pointing has higher arm elevations than pointing blindfolded. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37(5), 1485–1491. doi:10.1037/a0024232.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zhen, W., & Gros-Louis, J. (2015). Caregivers provide more labeling responses to infants’ pointing than to infants’ object-directed vocalizations. Journal of Child Language, 42(3), 538–561. doi:10.1017/S0305000914000221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank Georg Schüssler for technical support, Hanna Mathew and Felicitas Muth for assistance with the data collection.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Oliver Herbort.

Ethics declarations

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1 (DOCX 41 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Herbort, O., Kunde, W. How to point and to interpret pointing gestures? Instructions can reduce pointer–observer misunderstandings. Psychological Research 82, 395–406 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-016-0824-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-016-0824-8

Navigation