Skip to main content
Log in

Route planning with transportation network maps: an eye-tracking study

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Planning routes using transportation network maps is a common task that has received little attention in the literature. Here, we present a novel eye-tracking paradigm to investigate psychological processes and mechanisms involved in such a route planning. In the experiment, participants were first presented with an origin and destination pair before we presented them with fictitious public transportation maps. Their task was to find the connecting route that required the minimum number of transfers. Based on participants’ gaze behaviour, each trial was split into two phases: (1) the search for origin and destination phase, i.e., the initial phase of the trial until participants gazed at both origin and destination at least once and (2) the route planning and selection phase. Comparisons of other eye-tracking measures between these phases and the time to complete them, which depended on the complexity of the planning task, suggest that these two phases are indeed distinct and supported by different cognitive processes. For example, participants spent more time attending the centre of the map during the initial search phase, before directing their attention to connecting stations, where transitions between lines were possible. Our results provide novel insights into the psychological processes involved in route planning from maps. The findings are discussed in relation to the current theories of route planning.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7
Fig. 8
Fig. 9
Fig. 10

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bailenson, J. N., Shum, M. S., & Uttal, D. H. (1998). Road climbing: Principles governing asymmetric route choices on maps. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 18(3), 251–264. doi:10.1006/jevp.1998.0095.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bailenson, J. N., Shum, M. S., & Uttal, D. H. (2000). The initial segment strategy: A heuristic for route selection. Memory and Cognition, 28(2), 306–318. doi:10.3758/BF03213808.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beatty, J. (1982). Task-evoked pupillary responses, processing load, and the structure of processing resources. Psychological Bulletin, 91(2), 276–292.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beck, M. R., Trenchard, M., van Lamsweerde, A., Goldstein, R. R., & Lohrenz, M. (2012). Searching in clutter: Visual attention strategies of expert pilots. Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting, 56, 1411–1415. doi:10.1177/1071181312561400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benshoof, J. A. (1970). Characteristics of drivers’ route selection behavior. Traffic Engineering and Control, 11, 604–606.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bovy, P. H. L., & Stern, E. (1990). Route choice: Wayfinding in transport networks. Transportation Research Part A: Policy and Practice, 27, 338–339. doi:10.1007/978-94-009-033-4.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brunyé, T. T., Andonova, E., Meneghetti, C., Noordzij, M. L., Pazzaglia, F., Wienemann, R., … Taylor, H. A. (2012a). Planning routes around the world: International evidence for southern route preferences. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 32(4), 297–304. doi:10.1016/j.jenvp.2012.05.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brunyé, T. T., Gagnon, S. A., Waller, D., Hodgson, E., Tower-Richardi, S., & Taylor, H. A. (2012b). Up north and down south: Implicit associations between topography and cardinal direction. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(10), 1880–1894. doi:10.1080/17470218.2012.663393.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brunyé, T. T., Mahoney, C. R., Gardony, A. L., & Taylor, H. A. (2010). North is up(hill): Route planning heuristics in real-world environments. Memory and Cognition, 38(6), 700–712. doi:10.3758/MC.38.6.700.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brunyé, T. T., & Taylor, H. A. (2009). When goals constrain: Eye movements and memory for goal-oriented map study. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 23(6), 772–787. doi:10.1002/acp.1508.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Casakin, H., Barkowsky, T., Klippel, A., & Freksa, C. (2000). Schematic maps as way finding aids. In C. Freksa, C. Habel, & K. F. Wender (Eds.), Spatial cognition II: An interdisciplinary approach to representing and processing spatial knowledge (pp. 54–71). Berlin: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Cazzato, V., Basso, D., Cutini, S., & Bisiacchi, P. (2010). Gender differences in visuospatial planning: An eye movements study. Behavioural Brain Research, 206(2), 177–183. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2009.09.010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chowdhury, S., & Ceder, A. (2013). The effect of interchange attributes on public-transport users’ intention to use routes involving transfers. Psychology and Behavioral Sciences, 2(1), 5–13. doi:10.11648/j.pbs.20130201.12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conroy Dalton, R. (2003). The secret is to follow your nose: Route path selection and angularity. Environment and Behavior, 35(1), 107–131. doi:10.1177/0013916502238867.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Condappa, O., & Wiener, J. (2016). Human place and response learning: Navigation strategy selection, pupil size and gaze behavior. Psychological Research, 80(1), 82–93.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ettema, D., Friman, M., Gärling, T., Olsson, L. E., & Fujii, S. (2012). How in-vehicle activities affect work commuters’ satisfaction with public transport. Journal of Transport Geography, 24, 215–222. doi:10.1016/j.jtrangeo.2012.02.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farr, A. C., Kleinschmidt, T., Yarlagadda, P., & Mengersen, K. (2012). Wayfinding: A simple concept, a complex process. Transport Reviews, 32(6), 715–743. doi:10.1080/01441647.2012.712555.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freksa, C. (1999). Spatial aspects of task-specific wayfinding maps: A representation-theoretic perspective. In J. S. Gero & B. Tversky (Eds.), Visual and spatial reasoning in design (pp. 15–32). Sydney: Key Centre of Design Computing and Cognition, University of Sydney.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friman, M. (2010). Affective dimensions of the waiting experience. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 13(3), 197–205. doi:10.1016/j.trf.2010.04.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garland, H. C., Haynes, J. J., & Grubb, G. C. (1979). Transit map color coding and street detail. Environment and Behavior, 11(2), 162–184. doi:10.1177/0013916579112002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gärling, T., & Gärling, E. (1988). Distance minimization in downtown pedestrian shopping. Environment and Planning A, 20, 547–554. doi:10.1068/a200547.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gärling, T., Lindberg, E., & Mäntylä, T. (1983). Orientation in buildings: Effects of familiarity, visual access, and orientation aids. Journal of Applied Psychology, 68, 177–186. doi:10.1037/00219010.68.1.177.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gärling, T., Säisä, J., Book, A., & Lindberg, E. (1986). The spatiotemporal sequencing of everyday activities in the large-scale environment. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 6(4), 261–280. doi:10.1016/S0272-4944(86)80001-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Golledge, R. (1995). Path selection and route preference in human navigation: A progress report. In A. U. Frank & W. Kuhn (Eds.), Spatial information theory: A theoretical basis for GIS (COSIT’95). Lecture notes in computer science, no. 988 (pp. 207–222). Berlin: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Golledge, R. G. (1999). Human cognitive maps and wayfinding. In R. G. Golledge (Ed.), Wayfinding behavior: Cognitive mapping and other spatial processes (pp. 1–45). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guo, Z. (2011). Mind the map! The impact of transit maps on path choice in public transit. Transportation Research Part A: Policy and Practice, 45(7), 625–639. doi:10.1016/j.tra.2011.04.001.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes-Roth, B., & Hayes-Roth, F. (1979). A cognitive model of planning. Cognitive Science, 3(4), 275–310. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog0304_1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, J. M., Weeks, P. A, Jr., & Hollingworth, A. (1999). The effects of semantic consistency on eye movements during complex scene viewing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25(1), 210. doi:10.1037/00961523.25.1.210.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hine, J., & Scott, J. (2000). Seamless, accessible travel: Users’ views of the public transport journey and interchange. Transport Policy, 7(3), 217–226. doi:10.1016/S0967-070X(00)00022-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hölscher, C., Tenbrink, T., & Wiener, J. M. (2011). Would you follow your own route description? Cognitive strategies in urban route planning. Cognition, 121(2), 228–247. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2011.06.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). A theory of reading: From eye fixations to comprehension. Psychological Review, 87(4), 329–354. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.87.4.829.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacGregor, J. N., Chronicle, E. P., & Ormerod, T. C. (2004). Convex hull or crossing avoidance? Solution heuristics in the traveling salesperson problem. Memory and Cognition, 32(2), 260–270. doi:10.3758/BF03196857.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacGregor, J. N., & Chu, Y. (2011). Human performance on the traveling salesman and related problems: A review. The Journal of Problem Solving, 3(2), 1–29. doi:10.7771/1932-6246.1090.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacGregor, J. N., & Ormerod, T. (1996). Human performance on the traveling salesman problem. Perception and Psychophysics, 58(4), 527–539. doi:10.3758/BF03213088.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacGregor, J. N., Ormerod, T. C., & Chronicle, E. P. (1999). Spatial and contextual factors in human performance on the travelling salesperson problem. Perception, 28(11), 1417–1427. doi:10.1068/p2863.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meilinger, T., Hölscher, C., Büchner, S. J., & Brösamle, M. (2007). How much information do you need? Schematic maps in wayfinding and self localisation. In T. Barkowsky (Ed.), Spatial cognition V—reasoning, action, interaction (pp. 381–400). Berlin: Springer. doi:10.1007/978-3-540-75666-8_22.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Neider, M. B., & Zelinsky, G. J. (2006). Scene context guides eye movements during visual search. Vision Research, 46(5), 614–621. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2005.08.025.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pomplun, M., Reingold, E. M., & Shen, J. (2001). Investigating the visual span in comparative search: The effects of task difficulty and divided attention. Cognition, 81(2), B57–B67. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(01)00123-8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raveau, S., Guo, Z., Muñoz, J. C., & Wilson, N. H. M. (2014). A behavioural comparison of route choice on metro networks: Time, transfers, crowding, topology and socio-demographics. Transportation Research Part A: Policy and Practice, 66, 185–195. doi:10.1016/j.tra.2014.05.010.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, M. J. (2008). Underground maps after Beck (2nd ed.). Harrow Weald: Capital Transport Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, M. J., Newton, E. J., Lagattolla, F. D., Hughes, S., & Hasler, M. C. (2013). Objective versus subjective measures of Paris Metro map usability: Investigating traditional octolinear versus all-curves schematics. International Journal of Human Computer Studies, 71, 363–386. doi:10.1016/j.ijhcs.2012.09.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sobel, K. V., Gerrie, M. P., Poole, B. J., & Kane, M. J. (2007). Individual differences in working memory capacity and visual search: The roles of top-down and bottom-up processing. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 14(5), 840–845. doi:10.3758/BF03194109.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Orden, K. F., Limbert, W., Makeig, S., & Jung, T. P. (2001). Eye activity correlates of workload during a visuospatial memory task. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 43(1), 111–121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vickers, D., Lee, M. D., Dry, M., & Hughes, P. (2003). The roles of the convex hull and the number of potential intersections in performance on visually presented traveling salesperson problems. Memory and Cognition, 31(7), 1094–1104. doi:10.3758/BF03196130.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wiener, J. M., Ehbauer, N. N., & Mallot, H. A. (2009). Planning paths to multiple targets: Memory involvement and planning heuristics in spatial problem solving. Psychological Research, 73(5), 644–658. doi:10.1007/s00426-008-0181-3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wiener, J. M., & Tenbrink, T. (2008). Traveling salesman problem: The human case. Künstliche Intelligenz. KI und Kognition, 1(08), 18–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolfe, J. M. (1994). Guided Search 2.0 a revised model of visual search. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 1(2), 202–238. doi:10.3758/BF03200774.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This study was carried out thanks to a grant for 2 months international exchange from the Paris Descartes University Doctoral School (ED 261, Cognition, Comportements, Conduites Humaines).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Elise Grison.

Ethics declarations

Ethical approval

Ethical approval for the study was granted by the Bournemouth University committee. All procedures performed in this study were in accordance with 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments as well as comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that there were no conflicts of interest.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Grison, E., Gyselinck, V., Burkhardt, JM. et al. Route planning with transportation network maps: an eye-tracking study. Psychological Research 81, 1020–1034 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-016-0792-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-016-0792-z

Keywords

Navigation