Skip to main content
Log in

Attentional biases in ruminators and worriers

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study aims to investigate whether attentional biases typically associated with depression and anxiety already exist on a sub-clinical level. A transdiagnostic characteristic, both affective disorders have in common at a sub-clinical level, is persistent negative thinking (PNT), called rumination in depression and worrying in anxiety disorders. We investigated the association between these two types of PNT and attentional biases, using two different versions of the exogenous cueing tasks (ECT) in two different experiments. In Experiment 1, the cues of the ECT were negative and positive personality traits. This allowed us to investigate whether high-ruminators (N = 29), analogous to depressed patients, have difficulties to disengage attention from negative personality traits, as compared to low-ruminators (N = 40). In Experiment 2, the cues of the ECT were negative words related to themes participants frequently worry about versus positive words. This was done to investigate whether high-worriers (N = 26), analogous to anxious persons, have a strong tendency to automatically direct attention toward worry-related information, as compared to low-worriers (N = 27). The results of Experiment 1 showed that high-ruminators have difficulties to disengage their attention from negative personality traits. The results of Experiment 2 indicated that there were no attentional biases for high-worriers. These results show that the attentional bias typically associated with depression is already present at a sub-clinical level, whereas this seems not to be the case for the attentional bias typically associated with anxiety.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Correlation between raw RRS and PSWQ scores: r = .644, p < .001, overlap between groups, divided based on median split: 50 % (31 out of 62 students score low on both RRS and PSWQ or high on both RRS and PSWQ).

  2. None of the participants did not indicate any worry-theme as being relevant; two participants indicated that all of the worry-themes were personally relevant for them. On average, respondents indicated that two out of the four worry-themes were personally relevant to them.

  3. Correlation between raw RRS and PSWQ scores: r = .704, p < .001, overlap between groups, divided based on median split: 53 % (28 out of 53 students score low on both RRS and PSWQ or high on both RRS and PSWQ).

References

  • Ansseau, M., Fischler, B., Dierick, M., Albert, A., Leyman, S., & Mignon, A. (2008). Socioeconomic correlates of generalized anxiety disorder and major depression in primary care: the GADIS II study (Generalized Anxiety and Depression Impact Survey II). Depression and Anxiety, 25(6), 506–513.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and non anxious individuals: a meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin, 133(1), 1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Beck Depression Inventory-Second Edition (BDI-II). San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beckwé, M., Deroost, N., Koster, E. H., De Lissnyder, E., & De Raedt, R. (2013). Worrying and rumination are both associated with reduced cognitive control. Psychological Research, 1–10.

  • Borkovec, T. D., & Inz, J. (1990). The nature of worry in generalized anxiety disorder: a predominance of thought activity. Behavior Research and Therapy, 28, 153–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, B. P., Mogg, K., Falla, S. J., & Hamilton, L. R. (1998). Attentional bias for threatening facial expressions in anxiety: manipulation of stimulus duration. Cognition and Emotion, 12(6), 737–753.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, M. A. (2009). Investigating normal and pathological variation in memory-based inhibition: An examination of worry, thought suppression, and stimuli characteristics. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Texas at Austin), retrieved from http://repositories.lib.utexas.edu/bitstream/handle/2152/10592/brownm86568.pdf.

  • De Lissnyder, E., Koster, E. H. W., Goubert, L., Onraedt, T., Vanderhasselt, M., & De Raedt, R. (2012). Impaired cognitive control moderates the association between stress and rumination. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 43, 519–525.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Raedt, R., & Koster, E. H. (2010). Understanding vulnerability for depression from a cognitive neuroscience perspective: a reappraisal of attentional factors and a new conceptual framework. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 10(1), 50–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Donaldson, C., Lam, D., & Mathews, A. (2007). Rumination and attention in major depression. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45(11), 2664–2678.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, E. (2002). Processing emotional facial expressions: the role of anxiety and awareness. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 2(1), 52–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, E., Russo, R., & Dutton, K. (2002). Attentional bias for threat: evidence for delayed disengagement from emotional faces. Cognition and Emotion, 16(3), 355–379.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hakamata, Y., Lissek, S., Bar-Haim, Y., Britton, J. C., Fox, N. A., Leibenluft, E., … et al. (2010). Attention bias modification treatment: a meta-analysis toward the establishment of novel treatment for anxiety. Biological Psychiatry, 68(11), 982–990.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hermans, D., & De Houwer, J. (1994). Affective and subjective familiarity ratings of 740 Dutch words. Psychologica Belgica, 34, 115–140.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kerkhof, A., Hermans, D., Figee, A., Laeremans, I., Pieters, G., & Aardema, A. (2000). De Penn State Worry Questionnaire en de Worry Domains Questionnaire: Eerste resultaten bij Nederlandse en Vlaamse klinische en poliklinische populaties (The Penn State Worry Questionnaire and the Worry Domains Questionnaire: first results in Dutch and Flemish clinical populations). Gedragstherapie, 33, 135–146.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kessler, R. C., Gruber, M., Hettema, J. M., Hwang, I., Sampson, N., & Yonkers, K. A. (2008). Co-morbid major depression and generalized anxiety disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey follow-up. Psychological Medicine, 38(3), 365.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koster, E. H., Crombez, G., Verschuere, B., Van Damme, S., & Wiersema, J. R. (2006). Components of attentional bias to threat in high trait anxiety: facilitated engagement, impaired disengagement, and attentional avoidance. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44(12), 1757–1771.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koster, E. H., De Lissnyder, E., Derakshan, N., & De Raedt, R. (2011). Understanding depressive rumination from a cognitive science perspective: the impaired disengagement hypothesis. Clinical Psychology Review, 31(1), 138–145.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koster, E. H., De Raedt, R., Goeleven, E., Franck, E., & Crombez, G. (2005). Mood-congruent attentional bias in dysphoria: maintained attention to and impaired disengagement from negative information. Emotion, 5(4), 446.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koster, E. H., De Raedt, R., Leyman, L., & De Lissnyder, E. (2010). Mood-congruent attention and memory bias in dysphoria: exploring the coherence among information-processing biases. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48(3), 219–225.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lipp, O. V., & Derakshan, N. (2005). Attentional bias to pictures of fear-relevant animals in a dot probe task. Emotion, 5(3), 365.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, C., Rutherford, E., Campbell, L., Ebsworthy, G., & Holker, L. (2002). Selective attention and emotional vulnerability: assessing the causal basis of their association through the experimental manipulation of attentional bias. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111(1), 107.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, T. J., Miller, M. L., Metzger, R. L., & Borkovec, T. D. (1990). Development and validation of the Penn State Worry Questionnaire. Behavior Research and Therapy, 28, 487–495.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (1998). A cognitive-motivational analysis of anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36(9), 809–848.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (2005). Attentional bias in generalized anxiety disorder versus depressive disorder. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 29(1), 29–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their effects on the duration of depressive episodes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 569–582.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oathes, D. J., Siegle, G. J., & Ray, W. J. (2011). Chronic worry and the temporal dynamics of emotional processing. Emotion, 11(1), 101.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ormel, J., Von Korff, M., Ustun, T. B., Pini, S., Korten, A., & Oldehinkel, T. (1994). Common mental disorders and disability: a strong and cross-culturally consistent relationship. Results from the WHO Collaborative Study. JAMA, 272, 1741–1748.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. The Quarterly journal of Experimental Psychology, 32(1), 3–25. doi:10.1080/00335558008248231.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., & Cohen, Y. (1984). Components of visual orienting. Attention and performance X: control of language processes, 32, 531–556.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pratt, P., Tallis, F., & Eysenck, M. (1997). Information-processing, storage characteristics and worry. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35(11), 1015–1023.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raes, F., Schoofs, H., Hoes, D., Hermans, D., Van Den Eede, F., & Franck, E. (2009). ‘Reflection’ en ‘brooding’ als subtypes van rumineren: eenherziening van de Ruminative Response Scale (‘reflection’ and ‘brooding’ as subtypes of rumination: a revision of the ruminative response scale). Gedragstherapie, 42(3/4), 205–214.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reidy, J., & Richards, A. (1997). A memory bias for threat in high-trait anxiety. Personality and Individual Differences, 23(4), 653–663.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime reference guide. Pittsburgh: Psychology Software Tools Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Segal, Z. V., Gemar, M., Truchon, C., Guirguis, M., & Horowitz, L. M. (1995). A priming methodology for studying self-representation in major depressive disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104(1), 205.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Segerstrom, S. C., Tsao, J. C., Alden, L. E., & Craske, M. G. (2000). Worry and rumination: repetitive thought as a concomitant and predictor of negative mood. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 24(6), 671–688.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegle, G. J., Steinhauer, S. R., Carter, C. S., Ramel, W., & Thase, M. E. (2003). Do the seconds turn into hours? Relationships between sustained pupil dilation in response to emotional information and self-reported rumination. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27(3), 365–382.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R., Vagg, P. R., & Jacobs, G. A. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Rumination reconsidered: a psychometric analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27, 247–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van der Does, A. J. W. (2002). Handleiding: De Nederlandse versie van de Beck Depression Inventory, 2de Editie (the Dutch version of the Beck Depression Inventory (2nd ed.). Lisse: Swets and Zeitlinger B.V.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van der Ploeg, H. M. (2000). Handleiding bij de Zelf-Beoordelings Vragenlijst (the Dutch version of the STAI). Amsterdam: Harcourt Assessment B.V.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Rijsoort, S., Emmelkamp, P., & Vervaeke, G. (1999). The Penn StateWorry Questionnaire and the Worry Domains Questionnaire: structure, reliability and validity. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 6, 297–307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Rijsoort, S. N., Vervaeke, G. A. C., & Emmelkamp, P. M. G. (1997). De Penn State Worry Questionnaire en de Worry Domains Questionnaire: eerste resultaten bij een normale Nederlandse populatie (The Penn State Worry Questionnaire and the Worry Domains Questionnaire: first results in a Dutch population). Gedragstherapie, 30, 121–128.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verkuil, B., Brosschot, J. F., Putman, P., & Thayer, J. F. (2009). Interacting effects of worry and anxiety on attentional disengagement from threat. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47(2), 146–152.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wicks-Nelson, R., & Israel, A. C. (2009). Abnormal child and adolescent psychology. Upper Saddle River: Pearson Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yiend, J., & Mathews, A. (2001). Anxiety and attention to threatening pictures. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section A, 54(3), 665–681.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Mr. Thomas Kessels for language help and proofreading the article. The first author, Mieke Beckwé, is funded by the research counsel of the Vrije Universiteit Brussel (project 1582 BOF). There is no interest to be declared by the authors.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mieke Beckwé.

Appendix

Appendix

English translation of the personality traits and worry words used in respectively Experiment 1 and 2

 

Negative words

Positive words

Personality traits

Cowardly

Brutal

Happy

Honest

Asocial

Rude

Hopeful

Positive

Irritating

Frustrated

Sympathetic

Friendly

False

Jealous

Smart

Sincere

Unhealthy

Annoying

Polite

Creative

Suspicious

Unreliable

Relax

Enthusiastic

Cruel

Complaining

Alert

Nice

Lonely

Loveless

Energetic

Lively

Depressed

Doubtful

Helpful

Optimistic

Bossy

Hateful

Healthy

Funny

Liar

Selfish

Cheerful

Pleasant

Unpleasant

Pessimistic

Understanding

Righteous

Mean

Hostile

Active

Social

Aggressive

Unhappy

Thankful

Original

Indecisive

Intolerant

Reliable

Merry

Worry words

Fear of failure a

Relationships a

Cat

Television

Anxiety

Separation

Room

Consistent

Exams

Jealous

Entertaining

Juice

Nervous

Lonely

Objectively

Exhibition

Incompetent

Unreliable

Sewing machine

Sophisticated

Losses a

Health a

Pants

Direct

Grief

Cancer

Decent

Adventurous

Funeral

Illness

Firm

Pen

Sorry

Unhealthy

  

Depressed

Unattractive

  
  1. aNegative words are arranged according to worry-theme

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Beckwé, M., Deroost, N. Attentional biases in ruminators and worriers. Psychological Research 80, 952–962 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-015-0703-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-015-0703-8

Keywords

Navigation