Advertisement

Psychological Research

, Volume 80, Issue 5, pp 877–888 | Cite as

Investigating the effectiveness of working memory training in the context of Personality Systems Interaction theory

  • Tomáš Urbánek
  • Vladimír MarčekEmail author
Original Article

Abstract

Previous research has shown mixed results for the ability of working memory training to improve fluid intelligence. The aims of this study were first to replicate these improvements, and then to explore the moderating role of Personality Systems Interaction (PSI) personality factors. By using three different training methods and an active-contact control group, we examined the effects of 25 days of cognitive training on 142 participants. After examining our results in context of PSI theory, we found that different training methods yielded different IQ gains in participants, depending on their personality styles. In addition, these correlations suggested a meaningful pattern, indicating that PSI theory may be able to account for the different outcomes of cognitive training studies. Our findings may facilitate tailor-made cognitive training interventions in the future, and can contribute to explaining the mechanisms underlying the far transfer of working memory training to fluid intelligence.

Keywords

Personality Trait Cognitive Training Fluid Intelligence Personality Style Mental Rotation Task 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Notes

Acknowledgments

Our study took place with the financial support of the Czech Science Foundation, project no. GAČR13-36836S, awarded to Tomáš Urbánek. This funding source had no involvement in study design; in the collection, analysis, and interpretation of data; in the writing of the report; or in the decision to submit the article for publication.

References

  1. Ashton, M. C., & Lee, K. (2009). The HEXACO-60: A short measure of the major dimensions of personality. Journal of Personality Assessment, 91(4), 340–345.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Au, J., Sheehan, E., Tsai, N., Duncan, G. J., Buschkuehl, M., & Jaeggi, S. M. (2014). Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,. doi: 10.3758/s13423-014-0699-x.Google Scholar
  3. Baniqued, P. L., Kranz, M. B., Voss, M. W., Lee, H., Cosman, J. D., Severson, J., et al. (2013). Cognitive training with casual video games: Points to consider. Frontiers in Psychology,. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.01010.Google Scholar
  4. Bauernschmidt, A., Conway, C. M., & Pisoni, D. B. (2009). Working memory training and implicit learning. Report: Research on Spoken Language Processing Progress. 29.Google Scholar
  5. Brehmer, Y., Westerberg, H., & Bäckman, L. (2012). Working-memory training in younger and older adults: Training gains, transfer, and maintenance. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience,. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00063.Google Scholar
  6. Burgess, G. C., Gray, J. R., Conway, A. R. A., & Braver, T. S. (2011). Neural mechanisms of interference control underlie the relationship between fluid intelligence and working memory span. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140(4), 674–692.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Carretti, B., Borella, E., Zavagnin, M., & de Beni, R. (2012). Gains in language comprehension relating to working memory training in healthy older adults. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 28(5), 539–546.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Carroll, J. B. (1993). Human cognitive abilities: A survey of factor-analytic studies. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Chein, J. M., & Morrison, A. B. (2010). Expanding the mind’s workspace: Training and transfer effects with a complex working memory span task. Psychonomic Bulletin Review, 17(2), 193–199.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Chooi, W.-T., & Thompson, L. A. (2012). Working memory training does not improve intelligence in healthy young adults. Intelligence, 40(6), 531–542.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Colom, R., Abad, F. J., Quiroga, M. Á., Shih, P. C., & Flores-Mendoza, C. (2008). Working memory and intelligence are highly related constructs, but why? Intelligence, 36(6), 584–606.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Colom, R., Quiroga, M. Á., Shih, P. C., Martinez, K., Burgaleta, M., Martinez-Molina, A., et al. (2010). Improvement in working memory is not related to increased intelligence scores. Intelligence, 38(5), 497–505.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Colom, R., Román, F. J., Abad, F. J., Shih, P. C., Privado, J., Froufe, M., et al. (2013). Adaptive n-back training does not improve fluid intelligence at the construct level: gains on individual tests suggest that training may enhance visuospatial processing. Intelligence, 41(5), 712–727.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Colquitt, J. A., LePine, J. A., & Noe, R. (2000). Toward an integrative theory of training motivation: A meta-analytic path of analysis of 20 years of research. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85(5), 678–707.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Demetriou, A., Kyriakides, L., & Avraamidou, C. (2003). The missing link in the relations between intelligence and personality. Journal of Research in Personality, 37(6), 547–581.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Eysenck, H. J. (1950). Dimensions of personality (Vol. 5). New Brunswick: Transaction Publishers.Google Scholar
  17. Farsides, T., & Woodfield, R. (2003). Individual differences and undergraduate academic success: The roles of personality, intelligence, and application. Personality and Individual Differences, 34(7), 1225–1243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Freedman, D. A. (2010). Statistical models and causal inference: A dialogue with the social sciences. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  19. Gates, N., & Valenzuela, M. (2010). Cognitive exercise and its role in cognitive function in older adults. Current Psychiatry Reports, 12(1), 20–27.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Granger, C. W. (1969). Investigating causal relations by econometric models and cross-spectral methods. Econometrica: Journal of the Econometric Society, 37(3), 424–438.Google Scholar
  21. Gray, J. R., Chabris, C. F., & Braver, T. S. (2003). Neural mechanisms of general fluid intelligence. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 316–322.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Green, C. S., Strobach, T., & Schubert, T. (2014). On methodological standards in training and transfer experiments. Psychological Research, 78(6), 756–772.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Hossiep, R., Turck, D., & Hasella, M. (1999). Bochumer Matrizentest (BOMAT) advanced. Göttingen: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
  24. Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Jonides, J., & Perrig, W. J. (2008). Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(19), 6829.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Jonides, J., & Shah, P. (2011). Short- and long-term benefits of cognitive training. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(25), 10081–10086.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Shah, P., & Jonides, J. (2014). The role of individual differences in cognitive training and transfer. Memory & Cognition, 42(3), 464–480.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Jaeggi, S. M., Studer-Luethi, B., Buschkuehl, M., Su, Y.-F., Jonides, J., & Perrig, W. J. (2010). The relationship between n-back performance and matrix reasoning — implications for training and transfer. Intelligence, 38(6), 625–635.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Jausovec, N., & Jausovec, K. (2012a). Sex differences in mental rotation and cortical activation patterns: Can training change them? Intelligence, 40(2), 151–162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Jausovec, N., & Jausovec, K. (2012b). Working memory training: improving intelligence-changing brain activity. Brain and Cognition, 79(2), 96–106.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Johnson, W., & Bouchard, T. J. (2005a). Constructive replication of the visual-perceptual-image rotation model in Thurstone’s (1941) battery of 60 tests of mental ability. Intelligence, 33(4), 417–430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Johnson, W., & Bouchard, T. J. (2005b). The structure of human intelligence: It is verbal, perceptual, and image rotation (VPR), not fluid and crystallized. Intelligence, 33(4), 393–416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Kane, M. J., Hambrick, D. Z., & Conway, A. R. A. (2005). Working memory capacity and fluid intelligence are strongly related constructs: Comment on Ackerman, Beier, and Boyle (2005). Psychological Bulletin, 131(1), 66–71.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Karbach, J., & Kray, J. (2009). How useful is executive control training? Age differences in near and far transfer of task-switching training. Developmental Science, 12(6), 978–990.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Karbach, J., & Verhaeghen, P. (2014). Making working memory work: A meta-analysis of executive-control and working memory training in older adults. Psychological Science, 25(11), 2027–2037.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  35. Kaufman, S. B., DeYoung, C. G., Gray, J. R., Brown, J., & Mackintosh, N. (2009). Associative learning predicts intelligence above and beyond working memory and processing speed. Intelligence, 37(4), 374–382.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Kazén, M., Kuhl, J., Boermans, S., & Koole, S. L. (2013). Excelling at selling: The charming personality style predicts occupational activities, sales performance, and persuasive competence. PsyCh Journal, 2(2), 86–100.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Könen, T., & Karbach, J. (2015). The benefits of looking at intraindividual dynamics in cognitive training data. Frontiers in Psychology,. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00615.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  38. Kueider, A. M., Parisi, J. M., Gross, A. L., & Rebok, G. W. (2012). Computerized cognitive training with older adults: a systematic review. PLoS One, 7(7), e40588.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  39. Kuhl, J. (2000a). A theory of self-development: affective fixation and the STAR model of personality disorders and related styles. In J. Heckhausen (Ed.), Motivational psychology of human development: developing motivation and motivating development. Oxford: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  40. Kuhl, J. (2000b). The volitional basis of personality systems interaction theory: Applications in learning and treatment contexts. International Journal of Educational Research, 33(7), 665–703.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Kuhl, J. (2000c). A functional-design approach to motivation and volition: The dynamics of personality systems interactions. In M. Boekaerts, P. R. Pintrich, M. Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation (pp. 111–169). New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Kuhl, J., & Kazén, M. (1997). Persönlichkeits-Stil-und-Störungs-Inventar (PSSI) [The Personality-Styles-and-Disorder-Inventory (PSDI): Manual]. Göttingen: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
  43. Kuhl, J., Kazén, M., & Koole, S. L. (2006). Putting self-regulation theory into practice: A user’s manual. Applied Psychology, 55(3), 408–418.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Kuhl, J., & Koole, S. L. (2004). Workings of the will: A functional approach. In J. Greenberg, S. L., Koole, & T. Pyszczynski (Eds.), Handbook of experimental  existential psychology (pp. 411–430). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  45. Mackintosh, N. (2011). IQ and human intelligence. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  46. Makel, M. C., Plucker, J. A., & Hegarty, B. (2012). Replications in psychology research: how often do they really occur? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(6), 537–542.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Melby-Lervåg, M., & Hulme, C. (2013). Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic review. Developmental Psychology, 49(2), 270.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Moody, D. E. (2009). Can intelligence be increased by training on a task of working memory? Intelligence, 37(4), 327–328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Morrison, A. B., & Chein, J. M. (2011). Does working memory training work? The promise and challenges of enhancing cognition by training working memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(1), 46–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Oberauer, K., Süs, H. M., Wilhelm, O., & Wittmann, W. W. (2008). Which working memory functions predict intelligence? Intelligence, 36(6), 641–652.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Oelhafen, S., Nikolaidis, A., Padovani, T., Blaser, D., Koenig, T., & Perrig, W. J. (2013). Increased parietal activity after training of interference control. Neuropsychologia, 51(13), 2781–2790.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Owen, A. M., Hampshire, A., Grahn, J. A., Stenton, R., Dajani, S., Burns, A. S., et al. (2010). Putting brain training to the test. Nature, 465(7299), 775–778.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  53. Penner, I.-K., Vogt, A., Stöcklin, M., Gschwind, L., Opwis, K., & Calabrese, P. (2012). Computerised working memory training in healthy adults: a comparison of two different training schedules. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 22(5), 716–733.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. Peters, M., Laeng, B., Latham, K., Jackson, M., Zaiyouna, R., & Richardson, C. (1995). A redrawn Vandenberg and Kuse mental rotations test-different versions and factors that affect performance. Brain and Cognition, 28(1), 39–58.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  55. Pugin, F., Metz, A. J., Stauffer, M., Wolf, M., Jenni, O. G., & Huber, R. (2014). Working memory training shows immediate and long-term effects on cognitive performance in children and adolescents. F1000Research, 3, 82.Google Scholar
  56. Rabipour, S., & Raz, A. (2012). Training the brain: Fact and fad in cognitive and behavioral remediation. Brain and Cognition, 79(2), 159–179.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  57. Rafi, A., & Samsudin, K. (2009). Practising mental rotation using interactive desktop mental rotation trainer (iDeMRT). British Journal of Educational Technology, 40(5), 889–900.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Raven, J. C. (1990). Advanced Progressive Matrices. Sets I, II. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  59. Redick, T. S. (2015). Working memory training and interpreting interactions in intelligence interventions. Intelligence, 50, 14–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Redick, T. S., Shipstead, Z., Harrison, T. L., Hicks, K. L., Fried, D. E., Hambrick, D. Z., et al. (2012). No evidence of intelligence improvement after working memory training: A randomized, placebo-controlled study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(2), 359–379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Reijnders, J., van Heugten, C., & van Boxtel, M. (2012). Cognitive interventions in healthy older adults and people with mild cognitive impairment: A systematic review. Ageing Research Reviews, 12(1), 263–275.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  62. Salminen, T., Strobach, T., & Schubert, T. (2012). On the impacts of working memory training on executive functioning. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 166.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  63. Savage, L. (2013). Near and far transfer of working memory training related gains in healthy adults (Unpublished doctoral dissertation), Department of Clinical Psychology. Alberta: University of Calgary.Google Scholar
  64. Schmiedek, F., Lövdén, M., & Lindenberger, U. (2010). Hundred days of cognitive training enhance broad cognitive abilities in adulthood: Findings from the COGITO study. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 2, 27.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  65. Schubert, T., & Strobach, T. (2012). Video game experience and optimized executive control skills—On false positives and false negatives: Reply to Boot and Simons (2012). Acta Psychologica, 141(2), 278–280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Schwaighofer, M., Fischer, F., & Bühner, M. (2015). Does Working Memory Training Transfer? A Meta-Analysis Including Training Conditions as Moderators. Educational Psychologist, 50(2), 138–166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Shipstead, Z., Redick, T. S., & Engle, R. W. (2012). Is working memory training effective? Psychological Bulletin, 138(4), 628–654. doi: 10.1037/a0027473.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  68. Soubelet, A., & Salthouse, T. A. (2011). Personality–cognition relations across adulthood. Developmental Psychology, 47(2), 303.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  69. Spielberger, C. D., & Gorsuch, R. L. (1983). State-trait anxiety inventory for adults: Sampler set: manual, test, scoring key. Menlo Park: Mind Garden.Google Scholar
  70. Stephenson, C. (2010). Does training to increase working memory capacity improve fluid intelligence? (Doctoral dissertation). UMI Dissertation Publishing (number: 3417011). Claremont Graduate University, Ann Arbor, Michigan.Google Scholar
  71. Stephenson, C. L., & Halpern, D. F. (2013). Improved matrix reasoning is limited to training on tasks with a visuospatial component. Intelligence, 41(5), 341–357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Sternberg, R. J. (2008). Increasing fluid intelligence is possible after all. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(19), 6791–6792.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Stransky, D., Wilcox, L. M., & Dubrowski, A. (2010). Mental rotation: cross-task training and generalization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 16(4), 349.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  74. Studer-Luethi, B., Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., & Perrig, W. J. (2012). Influence of neuroticism and conscientiousness on working memory training outcome. Personality and Individual Differences, 53(1), 44–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Subramaniam, K., Luks, T. L., Fisher, M., Simpson, G. V., Nagarajan, S., & Vinogradov, S. (2012). Computerized cognitive training restores neural activity within the reality monitoring network in schizophrenia. Neuron, 73(4), 842–853.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  76. Thompson, T. W., Waskom, M. L., Garel, K.-L. A., Cardenas-Iniguez, C., Reynolds, G. O., Winter, R., et al. (2013). Failure of working memory training to enhance cognition or intelligence. PLoS One, 8(5), e63614.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  77. Wang, Z., Zhou, R., & Shah, P. (2014). Spaced cognitive training promotes training transfer. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 217.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  78. Wechsler, D. (2008). Wechsler adult intelligence scale–Fourth Edition (WAIS–IV). San Antonio: NCS Pearson.Google Scholar
  79. Wykes, T., Reeder, C., Landau, S., Everitt, B., Knapp, M., Patel, A., & Romeo, R. (2007). Cognitive remediation therapy in schizophrenia: Randomised controlled trial. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 190(5), 421–427.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2015

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Institute of PsychologyAcademy of Sciences of the Czech RepublicBrnoCzech Republic

Personalised recommendations