Skip to main content
Log in

Listening to music primes space: pianists, but not novices, simulate heard actions

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Musicians sometimes report twitching in their fingers or hands while listening to music. This anecdote could be indicative of a tendency for auditory-motor co-representation in musicians. Here, we describe two studies showing that pianists (Experiment 1), but not novices (Experiment 2) automatically generate spatial representations that correspond to learned musical actions while listening to music. Participants made one-handed movements to the left or right from a central location in response to visual stimuli while listening to task-irrelevant auditory stimuli, which were scales played on a piano. These task-irrelevant scales were either ascending (compatible with rightward movements) or descending (compatible with leftward movements). Pianists were faster to respond when the scale direction was compatible with the direction of response movement, whereas novices’ movements were unaffected by the scale. These results are in agreement with existing research on action–effect coupling in musicians, which draw heavily on common coding theory. In addition, these results show how intricate auditory stimuli (ascending or descending scales) evoke coarse, domain-general spatial representations.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Note that although a descending (ascending) scale must be played with leftward (rightward) hand-finger movements, the arm movements that precedes the scale is independent; a pianist might reach rightward to initiate a descending scale, which would be performed with leftward movements.

References

  • Altenmüller, E., & Schneider, S. (2009). Planning and performance. In S. Hallam, I. Cross & M. Thaut (Eds.), The Oxford Handbooks of Music Psychology (Ch. 31). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.

  • Bangert, M., & Altenmuller, E. O. (2003). Mapping perception to action in piano practice: a longitudinal DC-EEG study. BMC Neuroscience, 4, 26.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Calvo-Merino, B., Glaser, D. E., Grezes, J., Passingham, R. E., & Haggard, P. (2005). Action observation and acquired motor skills: an fMRI study with expert dancers. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 1243–1249.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cross, E, Hamilton, A F deC, & Grafton, S T (2006). Building a motor simulation de novo: Observation of dance by dancers. NeuroImage, 31, 1257–1267.

  • Drost, U., Rieger, M., Brass, M., Gunter, T., & Prinz, W. (2005a). Action-effect coupling in pianists. Psychological Research, 69, 233–241.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Drost, U., Rieger, M., Brass, M., Gunter, T., & Prinz, W. (2005b). When hearing turns into playing: Movement induction by auditory stimuli in pianists. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, Section A, 58, 1376–1389.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Drost, U., Riger, M., & Prinz, W. (2007). Instrument specificity in experienced musicians. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 527–533.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eitan, Z., & Granot, R. Y. (2006). How music moves: Musical parameters and listeners images of motion. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 23, 221–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freedberg, D., & Gallese, V. (2007). Motion, emotion and empathy in esthetic experience. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 197–203.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Godøy, R. I., & Leman, M. (Eds.). (2010). Musical gestures: Sound, movement, and meaning. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hallam, S., Cross, I., & Thaut, M. (Eds.). (2009). The Oxford handbook of music psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haslinger, B., Erhard, P., Altenmüller, E., Schroeder, U., Boecker, H., & Ceballos-Baumann, A. O. (2005). Transmodal sensorimotor networks during action observation in professional pianists. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 282–293.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Haueisen, J., & Knosche, T. (2001). Involuntary motor activity in pianists evoked by music perception. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 786–792.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hodges, D. A. (2009). Bodily responses to music. In S. Hallam, I. Cross & M. Thaut (Eds.), The Oxford Handbooks of Music Psychology (Ch. 11). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.

  • Hommel, B., Musseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–878.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Keller, P. E., Knoblich, G., & Repp, B. H. (2007). Pianists duet better when they play with themselves: on the possible role of action simulation in synchronization. Consciousness and Cognition, 16, 102–111.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kilner, J. M., Paulignan, Y., & Blakemore, S.-J. (2003). An interference effect of observed biological movement on action. Current Biology, 13, 522–525.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lahav, A., Saltzman, E., & Schlaug, G. (2007). Action representation of sound: audiomotor recognition network while listening to newly acquired actions. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 308–314.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leder, H., Bär, Siegrun, & Topolinski, S. (2012). Covert painting simulations influence aesthetic appreciation of artworks. Psychological Science, 23, 1479–1481.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lidji, P., Kolinsky, R., Lochy, A., & Morais, J. (2007). Spatial associations for musical stimuli: a piano in the head? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33, 1189.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lotze, M., Scheler, G., Tan, H.-R. M., Braun, C., & Birbaumer, N. (2003). The musician’s brain: functional imaging of amateurs and professionals during performance and imagery. NeuroImage, 20, 1817–1829.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mahon, B. Z., & Caramazza, A. (2008). A critical look at the embodied cognition hypothesis and a new proposal for grounding conceptual content. Journal of PhysiologyParis, 102, 59–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • Molnar-Szakacs, I., & Overy, K. (2006). Music and mirror neurons: From motion to ‘e’motion. SCAN, 1, 235–241.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Novembre, G., Ticini, L. F., Schütz-Bosbach, S., & Keller, P. E. (2013). Motor simulation and the coordination of self and other in real-time joint action. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

  • Pratt, C. C. (1930). The spatial character of high and low tones. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 13, 278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9, 129–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Repp, B. H., & Knoblich, G. (2007). Action can affect auditory perception. Psychological Science, 18, 6–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Repp, B. H., & Knoblich, G. (2009). Performed or observed keyboard actions affects pianists’ judgments of relative pitch. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 2156–2170.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rusconi, E., Kwan, B., Giordano, B. L., Umilta, C., & Butterworth, B. (2006). Spatial representation of pitch height: the SMARC effect. Cognition, 99, 113–129.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seashore, C. E. (1967). Psychology of Music (2nd ed.). Retrieved from http://books.google.com/books (Original work published 1938).

  • Simon, J. R., & Rudell, A. P. (1967). Auditory SR compatibility: the effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. Journal of Applied Psychology, 51(3), 300.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stewart, L., Walsh, V., & Frith, U. (2004). Reading music modifies spatial mapping in pianists. Perception and Psychophysics, 66, 183–195.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, J. E. T., Witt, J. K., & Grimaldi, P. G. (2012). Uncovering the connection between artist and audience: viewing painted brushstrokes evokes corresponding action representations in the observer. Cognition, 125, 26–36.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (1998). On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 830–846.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, S., Winner, E., Cicchetti, D., & Gardner, H. (1981). “Metaphorical” mapping in human infants. Child Development, 52, 728–731.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wühr, P., & Müsseler, J. (2002). Blindness to response-compatible stimuli in the psychological refractory period paradigm. Visual Cognition, 9, 421–457.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Jessica K. Witt was supported by a Grant from the National Science Foundation (BCS-0957051).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to J. Eric T. Taylor.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Taylor, J.E.T., Witt, J.K. Listening to music primes space: pianists, but not novices, simulate heard actions. Psychological Research 79, 175–182 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-014-0544-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-014-0544-x

Keywords

Navigation