Skip to main content
Log in

Acting in perspective: the role of body and language as social tools

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We investigated how the reach-to-grasp movement is influenced by the presence of another person (friend or non-friend), who was either invisible (behind) or located in different positions with respect to an object and to the agent, and by the perspective conveyed by linguistic pronouns (“I”, “You”). The interaction between social relationship and relative position influenced the latency of both maximal fingers aperture and velocity peak, showing shorter latencies in the presence of a non-friend than in the presence of a friend. However, whereas the relative position of a non-friend did not affect the kinematics of the movement, the position of a friend mattered: latencies were significantly shorter with friends only in positions allowing them to easily reach for the object. Finally, the investigation of the overall reaching movement time showed an interaction between the speaker and the pronoun: participants reached the object more quickly when the other spoke, particularly if she used the “I” pronoun. This suggests that speaking, and particularly using the “I” pronoun, evokes a potential action. Implications of the results for embodied cognition are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617–645.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Becchio, C., Sartori, L., Bulgheroni, M., & Castiello, U. (2008a). The case of Dr. Jekyll and Mr. Hyde: a kinematic study on social intention. Consciousness and Cognition, 17, 557–564.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Becchio, C., Sartori, L., Bulgheroni, M., & Castiello, U. (2008b). Both your intention and mine are reflected in the kinematics of my reach to grasp movement. Cognition, 106, 894–912.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Becchio, C., Sartori, L., & Castiello, U. (2010). Toward you: the social side of action. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19, 183–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bensoussan, L., Mesure, S., Viton, J. V., & Delarque, A. (2006). Kinematic and kinetic asymmetries in hemiplegic patients’ gait initiation patterns. Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine, 38, 287–294.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Binkofski, F., & Buccino, G. (2009). The role of ventral premotor cortex in action execution and action understanding. Journal of Physiology Paris, 99, 396–405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Borghi, A. M., & Cimatti, F. (2009). Words as tools and the problem of abstract words meanings. In N. Taatgen & H. van Rijn (Eds.), Proceedings of the 31st Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 2304–2309). Amsterdam: Cognitive Science Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borghi, A. M., & Cimatti, F. (2010). Embodied cognition and beyond: acting and sensing the body. Neuropsychologia, 48, 763–773.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Borghi, A. M., Gianelli, C., & Scorolli, C. (2010). Sentence comprehension: effectors and goals, self and others. An overview of experiments and implications for robotics. Frontiers in Neurorobotics, 4, 3.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Borghi, A. M., Glenberg, A. M., & Kaschak, M. P. (2004). Putting words in perspective. Memory and Cognition, 32(6), 863–873.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brunyé, T. T., Ditman, T., Mahoney, C. R., Augustyn, J. S., & Taylor, H. A. (2009). When you and I share perspectives: pronouns modulate perspective taking during narrative comprehension. Psychological Science, 20(1), 27–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bruzzo, A., Borghi, A. M., & Ghirlanda, S. (2008). Hand–object interaction in perspective. Neuroscience Letters, 441, 61–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Caligiore, D., Borghi, A.M., Parisi, D., Ellis, R., Cangelosi, A., Baldassarre, G. (2011). How Affordances Associated with a distractor Object Can Cause Compatibility Effects: A Study with the Computational Model TRoPICALS (accepted, this special issue).

  • Chatterjee, A. (2010). Disembodying cognition. Language and Cognition, 2–1, 79–116.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. (1998). Magic words: How language augments human computation. In P. Carruthers & J. Boucher (Eds.), Language and thought: interdisciplinary themes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Costantini, M., Ambrosini, E., Scorolli, C., & Borghi, A.M. (2011a). When objects are close to me: affordances in the peripersonal space. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 18, 32–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Costantini, M., Committeri, G., & Sinigaglia, C. (2011b). Ready both to your and to my hands: Mapping the reaching space of others. PLoSONE, 6(4), e17923.

  • D’Ausilio, A., Pulvermüller, F., Salmas, P., Bufalari, I., Begliomini, C., & Fadiga, L. (2009). The motor somatotopy of speech perception. Current Biology, 19, 381–385.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Daprati, E., Wriessnegger, S., & Lacquaniti, F. (2011). Dealing with individual variability: When telling what is real depends on telling who is acting. Neuroscience Letters, 498(1), 6–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, R., & Tucker, M. (2000). Micro-affordance: the potentiation of components of action by seen objects. British Journal of Psychology, 9, 451–471.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, R., Tucker, M., Symes, E., & Vainio, L. (2007). Does selecting one visual object from several require inhibition of the actions associated with nonselected objects? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33, 670–691.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ferri, F., Campione, G. C., Dalla Volta, R., Gianelli, C., & Gentilucci, M. (2011). Social requests and social affordances: how they affect the kinematics of motor sequences during interactions between conspecifics. PLoS ONE, 6(1), e15855.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ferri, F., Stoianov, I., Gianelli, C., D’Amico, L., Borghi, A. M., & Gallese, V. (2010). When action meets emotions. How facial displays of emotion influence goal-related behavior. PloSOne, 5(10), e13126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, M. H. (2005). Action simulation is not constrained by one’s own postures. Neuropsychologia, 43, 28–34.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, M. H., & Zwaan, R. A. (2008). Embodied language: a review of the role of the motor system in language comprehension. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (Colchester), 61(6), 825–850.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J. H., Green, F. L., & Flavell, E. R. (1986). Development of knowledge about the appearance–reality distinction. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 51(i–v), 1–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V. (2009). Motor abstraction: a neuroscientific account of how action goals and intentions are mapped and understood. Psychological Research, 73(4), 486–498.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain, 119(Pt 2), 593–609.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gentner, D. (2010). Psychology in cognitive science: 1978–2038. Topics in Cognitive Science, 2, 328–344.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Georgiou, J., Becchio, C., Glover, S., & Castiello, U. (2007). Different action patterns for cooperative and competitive behaviour. Cognition, 102, 415–433.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gianelli, C., Dalla Volta, R., Barbieri, F., & Gentilucci, M. (2008). Automatic grasp imitation following action observation affects estimation of intrinsic object properties. Brain Research, 1218, 166–180.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gianelli, C., Farnè, A., Salemme, R., Jeannerod, M., & Roy, A.C. (2011). The agent is right: when motor embodied cognition is space-dependent. PloSOne, 6(9), e25036.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston MA: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grèzes, J., Tucker, M., Armony, J., Ellis, R., & Passingham, R. E. (2003). Objects automatically potentiate action: an fMRI study of implicit processing. European Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 2735.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, A. F. D., Brindley, R., & Frith, U. (2009). Visual perspective taking impairment in children with autistic spectrum disorder. Cognition, 113, 37–44.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hartup, W.W. (1996). Cooperation, close relationships, and cognitive development. In The company they keep: Friendship in childhood and adolescence. Bukowski, William M. (Ed.); Newcomb, Andrew F. (Ed.); Hartup, Willard W. (Ed.) (pp. 213–237). New York, NY, US: Cambridge University Press.

  • Helbing, D., & Yu, W. (2009). The outbreak of cooperation among success-driven individuals under noisy conditions. Proceedings oft he National Academy of Science, 106, 3680–3685.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B., Colzato, L. S., & van den Wildenberg, W. P. M. (2009). How social are task representations? Psychological Science, 20(7), 794–798.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B., Musseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–878.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, P.L., Meltzoff, A.N., & Decety, J. (2006). Neural circuits involved in imitation and perspective-taking. Neuroimage. 31(1), 429–439.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jakobson, L. S., & Goodale, M. A. (1991). Factors affecting higher-order movement planning: a kinematic analysis of human prehension. Experimental Brain Research, 86, 199–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaschak, M. P., Madden, C. J., Therriault, D. J., Yaxley, R. H., Aveyard, M., Blanchard, A. A., et al. (2005). Perception of motion affects language processing. Cognition, 94, B79–B89.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kessler, K., & Rutherford, L.A. (2010). The two forms of visuo-spatial perspective taking are differently embodied and subserve different spatial prepositions. Frontiers in Cognition. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00213.

  • Kessler, K., & Thomson, L. A. (2010). The embodied nature of spatial perspective taking: embodied transformation versus sensorimotor interference. Cognition, 114, 72–88.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lyons, M. T., & Aitken, S. J. (2008) Machiavellianism in strangers affects cooperation. Journal of Evolutionary Psychology, 6, 173–185.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacWhinney, B. (2005). The emergence of grammar from perspective taking. In D. Pecher & R. Zwaan (Eds.), The grounding of cognition: the role of perception and action in memory, language, and thinking (pp. 198–223). Cambridge UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Marteniuk, R. G., Mackenzie, C. L., Jeannerod, M., Athenes, S., & Dugas, C. (1987). Constraints on human arm movement trajectories. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 41, 365–378.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, A. (2007). The representation of object concepts in the brain. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 25–45.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marzoli, D., Mitaritonna, A., Moretto, F., Carluccio, P., & Tommasi, L. (2011). The handedness of imagined bodies in action and the role of perspective taking. Brain and Cognition, 75, 51–59.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maslovat, D., Hodges, N. J., Chua, R., & Franks, I. M. (2011). Motor preparation of spatially and temporally defined movements: evidence from startle. Journal of Neurophysiology, 106, 885–894.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Massen, J.J.M, Sterck, E.H.M., & de Vos, H. (2010). Close social associations in animals and humans: Functions and mechanisms of friendship. Behaviour, 147, 1379–1412.

    Google Scholar 

  • Patchay, S., Castiello, U., & Haggard, P. (2003). A cross-modal interference effect in grasping objects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10, 924–931.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pezzulo, G., Barca, L., Lamberti Bocconi, A., & Borghi, A. M. (2010). When affordances climb into your mind: advantages of motor simulation in a memory task performed by novice and expert rock climbers. Brain and Cognition, 73, 68–73.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9, 129–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pulvermüller, F., & Fadiga, L. (2010). Active perception: sensorimotor circuits as a cortical basis for language. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 351–360.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Richardson, M. J., Marsh, K. L., & Baron, R. M. (2007). Judging and actualizing intrapersonal and interpersonal affordances. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33, 845–859.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rivas, J. (2009). Friendship selection. International Journal of Game Theory, 38, 521–538.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rueschemeyer, S. A., Lindemann, O., van Elk, M., & Bekkering, H. (2009). Embodied cognition: the interplay between automatic resonance and selection-for-action mechanisms. European Journal of Social Psychology, 39, 1180–1187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sartori, L., Becchio, C., Bulgheroni, M., & Castiello, U. (2009). Modulation of the action control system by social intention: unexpected social requests override preplanned action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 1490–1500.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schütz-Bosbach, S., Mancini, B., Aglioti, S. M., & Haggard, P. (2006). Self and other in the human motor system. Current Biology, 16(8), 1830–1834.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scorolli, C., Daprati, E., Nico, D., & Borghi, A. M. (2011). Language comprehension and space representation: words as tools. Budapest: Conference on Cognitive Development.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sebanz, N., Bekkering, H., & Knoblich, G. (2006). Joint action: bodies and minds moving together. Trends in Cognitive Science, 10, 70–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Semin, G. R., & Smith, E. R. (Eds.). (2008). Embodied grounding: Social, cognitive, affective, and neuroscientific approaches. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silk, J.B. (2003). Cooperation without counting: the puzzle of friendship. In P. Hammerstein (Ed.). The Genetic and Cultural Evolution of Cooperation. (pp. 37–54). Dahlem Workshop Report 90. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

  • Smith, P. J. (1996). Strategies of Co-operation: a Commentary. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 19, 81–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: the origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 675–735.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Toni, I., de Lange, F. P., Noordzij, M. L., & Hagoort, P. (2008). Language beyond action. Journal of Physiology Paris, 102(1–3), 71–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, B., & Hard, B. M. (2009). Embodied and disembodied cognition: Spatial perspective-taking. Cognition, 110, 124–129.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tylèn, K., Weed, E., Wallentin, M., Roepstorff, A., & Frith, C. D. (2010). Language as a tool for interacting minds. Mind and Language, 25, 3–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vogt, S., Taylor, P., & Hopkins, B. (2003). Visuomotor priming by pictures of hands: perspective matters. Neuropsychologia, 41, 941–951.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yoon, E. Y., Humphreys, W. W., & Riddoch, M. J. (2010). The paired-object affordance effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 812–824.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zajonc, R. B. (1965). Social facilitation. Science, 149, 269–274.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The first two authors contributed equally, and the order of their names was randomly selected. The first two authors designed the experiment, conducted the experiment, analyzed the data and wrote the paper. The third author designed the experiment and wrote the paper. This work was supported by the European Community, project ROSSI: Emergence of communication in RObots through Sensorimotor and Social Interaction (Grant agreement n. 216125).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Claudia Gianelli, Claudia Scorolli or Anna M. Borghi.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Gianelli, C., Scorolli, C. & Borghi, A.M. Acting in perspective: the role of body and language as social tools. Psychological Research 77, 40–52 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-011-0401-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-011-0401-0

Keywords

Navigation