Skip to main content
Log in

Anxiety and perceptual-motor performance: toward an integrated model of concepts, mechanisms, and processes

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Under anxiety, people sometimes perform poorly. This concerns cognitive performance (e.g., taking an important exam) as well as perceptual-motor performance (e.g., picking up a cup from a table). There is still much debate about how anxiety affects perceptual-motor performance. In the current paper we review the experimental literature on anxiety and perceptual-motor performance, thereby focusing on how anxiety affects the perception, selection, and realization of action possibilities. Based on this review we discuss the merits of two opposing theoretical explanations and build on existing frameworks of anxiety and cognitive performance to develop an integrated model that explains the various ways in which anxiety may specifically affect perceptual-motor performance. This model distinguishes between positive and negative effects of anxiety and, moving beyond previous approaches, recognizes three operational levels (i.e., attentional, interpretational, and behavioral) at which anxiety may affect different aspects of goal-directed action. Finally, predictions are formulated and directions for future research suggested.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Amir, N., Elias, J., Klumpp, H., & Przeworski, A. (2003). Attentional bias to threat in social phobia: Facilitated processing of threat or difficulty disengaging attention from threat? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 1325–1335.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & IJzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: A meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 1–24.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Behan, M., & Wilson, M. R. (2008). State anxiety and visual attention: The role of the quiet eye period in aiming to a far target. Journal of Sport Sciences, 26, 207–215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beilock, S. L. (2008a). Math performance in stressful situations. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 339–343.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beilock, S. L. (2008b). Beyond the playing field: Sport psychology meets embodied cognition. International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 1, 19–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beilock, S. L., & Carr, T. H. (2001). On the fragility of skilled performance: what governs choking under pressure? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 701–725.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beilock, S. L., Carr, T. H., MacMahon, C., & Starkes, J. L. (2002). When paying attention becomes counterproductive: Impact of divided versus skill-focused attention on novice and experienced performance of sensorimotor skills. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8, 6–16.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beilock, S. L., & Gray, R. (2007). Why do athletes choke under pressure? In G. Tenenbaum & R. C. Eklund (Eds.), Handbook of Sport Psychology (3rd Ed ed., pp. 425–444). Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bernstein, N. A. (1996). On dexterity and its development. In M. L. Latash & M. T. Turvey (Eds.), Dexterity and its development (pp. 3–246). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beuter, A., & Duda, J. L. (1985). Analysis of the arousal/motor performance relationship in children using movement kinematics. Journal of Sport Psychology, 7, 229–243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Binsch, O., Oudejans, R. R. D., Bakker, F. C., & Savelsbergh, G. J. P. (2010). Ironic effects and final target fixation in a penalty shooting task. Human Movement Science, 29, 277–288.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, S. J. (2007). Neurocognitive mechanisms of anxiety: An integrative account. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 307–316.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, S. J., Duncan, J., & Lawrence, A. D. (2004a). Prefrontal cortical function and anxiety: Controlling attention to threat-related stimuli. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 184–188.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, S. J., Duncan, J., & Lawrence, A. D. (2004b). State anxiety modulation of the amygdala response to unattended threat-related stimuli. Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 10364–10368.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blanchette, I., & Richards, A. (2010). The influence of affect on higher level cognition: A review of research on interpretation, judgement, decision making and reasoning. Cognition & Emotion, 24, 561–595.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, L. A., Doan, J. B., McKenzie, N. C., & Cooper, S. A. (2006). Anxiety-mediated gait adaptations reduce errors of obstacle negotiation among younger and older adults: Implications for fall risk. Gait and Posture, 24, 418–423.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Calvo, M. G., & Castillo, M. G. (2001). Bias in predictive inferences during reading. Discourse Processes, 32, 43–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Causer, J., Holmes, P. S., Smith, N. C., & Williams, A. M. (2011). Anxiety, movement kinematics, and visual attention in elite-level performers. Emotion, 11, 595–602.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Coombes, S. A., Higgins, T., Gamble, K. M., Cauraugh, J. H., & Janelle, C. M. (2009). Attentional control theory: Anxiety, emotion, and motor planning. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 23, 1072–1079.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Correll, J., Park, B., Judd, C. M., & Wittenbrink, B. (2002). The police officer’s dilemma: Using race to disambiguate potentially threatening individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1314–1329.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DeCaro, M. S., Thomas, R. D., Albert, N. B., & Beilock, S. L. (2011). Choking under pressure: Multiple routes to skill failure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140, 390–406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, M. W. (1992). Anxiety: the cognitive perspective. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, M. W., & Calvo, M. G. (1992). Anxiety and performance: The processing efficiency theory. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 409–434.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, M. W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., & Calvo, M. G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance: Attentional control theory. Emotion, 7, 336–353.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. (1983). The modularity of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT/Bradford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fox, E., Russo, R., Bowles, R., & Dutton, K. (2001). Do threatening stimuli draw or hold visual attention in subclinical anxiety? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 681–700.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frijda, N. H. (1988). The laws of emotions. American Psychologist, 43, 349–358.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gould, D., & Udry, E. (1994). Psychological skills for enhancing performance: Arousal regulation strategies. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 26, 478–485.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gray, R. (2004). Attending to the execution of a complex sensorimotor skill: Expertise differences, choking, and slumps. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 10, 42–54.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gucciardi, D. F., & Dimmock, J. A. (2008). Choking under pressure in sensorimotor skills: Conscious processing or depleted attentional resources? Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 9, 45–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gucciardi, D. F., Longbottom, J., Jackson, B., & Dimmock, J. A. (2010). Experienced golfers perspectives on choking under pressure. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 32, 61–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hardy, L. (1996). Testing the predictions of the cusp-catastrophe model of anxiety and perceptual-motor performance. Sport Psychologist, 10, 140–156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hatzigeorgiadis, A., & Biddle, S. J. H. (2000). Assessing cognitive interference in sport: Development of the thought occurrence questionnaire for sport. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 13, 65–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hatzigeorgiadis, A., & Biddle, S. J. H. (2001). Athletes’ perceptions of how cognitive interference during competition influences concentration and effort. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 14, 411–429.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, R. C., Ashford, K. J., & Norsworthy, G. (2006). Attentional focus, dispositional reinvestment, and skilled motor performance under pressure. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 28, 49–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jacobs, D. M., Runeson, S., & Michaels, C. F. (2001). Learning to visually perceive the relative mass of colliding balls in globally and locally constrained task ecologies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 1019–1038.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Janelle, C. M. (2002). Anxiety, arousal and visual attention: A mechanistic account of performance variability. Journal of Sports Sciences, 20, 237–251.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jostmann, N. B., & Koole, S. L. (2007). On the regulation of cognitive control: Action orientation moderates the impact of high demands in Stroop interference tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 593–609.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, H., Somerville, L. H., Johnstone, T., Polis, S., Alexander, A. L., et al. (2004). Contextual modulation of amygdala responsivity to surprised faces. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 1730–1745.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krieglmeyer, R., De Houwer, J., & Deutsch, R. (2011). How farsighted are behavioral tendencies of approach and avoidance? The effect of stimulus valence on immediate vs ultimate distance change. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 622–627.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krieglmeyer, R., Deutsch, R., De Houwer, J., & De Raedt, R. (2010). Being moved: Valence activates approach-avoidance behavior independently of evaluation and approach-avoidance intention. Psychological Science, 21, 607–613.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lam, W. K., Maxwell, J. P., & Masters, R. (2009). Analogy learning and the performance of motor skills under pressure. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 31, 337–357.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Land, M. F. (2009). Vision, eye movements and natural behavior. Visual Neuroscience, 26, 51–62.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lavender, T., & Hommel, B. (2007). Affect and action: Towards an event-coding account. Cognition and Emotion, 21, 1270–1296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Masters, R. S. W. (1992). Knowledge, knerves and know-how: The role of explicit versus implicit knowledge on the breakdown of a complex motor skill under pressure. British Journal of Psychology, 83, 334–358.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Masters, R. S. W. (1993). Reinvestment: A dimension of personality implicated in skill breakdown under pressure. Personality and Individual Differences, 14, 655–666.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mathews, A. (1990). Why worry? The cognitive function of anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 28, 455–468.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mathews, A., & Mackintosh, B. (1998). A cognitive model of selective processing in anxiety. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 22, 539–560.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nibbeling, N., Daanen, H. A. M., Gerritsma, R. M., Hofland, R. M., & Oudejans, R. R. D. (2011). Effects of anxiety on running. Journal of Sports Sciences (in press).

  • Nicholls, A. R., & Polman, R. C. J. (2007). Coping in sport: A systematic review. Journal of Sports Sciences, 25, 11–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nieuwenhuys, A., Hanin, Y. L., & Bakker, F. C. (2008). Performance related experiences and coping during races: A case of an elite sailor. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 9, 61–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nieuwenhuys, A., & Oudejans, R. R. D. (2010). Effects of anxiety on handgun shooting behavior of police officers: A pilot study. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 23, 225–233.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nieuwenhuys, A., & Oudejans, R. R. D. (2011). Training with anxiety: Short- and long-term effects on police officers’ shooting behavior under pressure. Cognitive Processing, 12, 277–288.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Nieuwenhuys, A., Pijpers, J. R., Oudejans, R. R. D., & Bakker, F. C. (2008). The influence of anxiety on visual attention in climbing. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 30, 171–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nieuwenhuys, A., Savelsbergh, G. J. P., Oudejans, R. R. D. (2011). Shoot or don’t shoot: Why police officers are more inclined to shoot when they are anxious. Emotion (in press).

  • Nieuwenhuys, A., Vos, L., Pijpstra, S., & Bakker, F. C. (2011). Meta experiences and coping effectiveness in sport. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 11, 135–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oudejans, R. R. D. (2008). Reality based practice under pressure improves handgun shooting performance of police officers. Ergonomics, 51, 261–273.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oudejans, R. R. D., Kuijpers, W., Kooiman, C. C., & Bakker, F. C. (2011). Thoughts and attention of athletes under pressure: skill-focus or performance worries? Anxiety, Stress and Coping, 24, 59–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oudejans, R. R. D., Michaels, C. F., Bakker, F. C., & Davids, K. (1999). Shedding some light on catching in the dark: Perceptual mechanisms for catching fly balls. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 531–542.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oudejans, R. R. D., & Nieuwenhuys, A. (2009). Perceiving and moving in sports and other high-pressure contexts. Progress in Brain Research, 174, 35–48.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oudejans, R. R. D., & Pijpers, J. R. (2009). Training with anxiety has a positive effect on expert perceptual-motor performance under pressure. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 1631–1647.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oudejans, R. R. D., & Pijpers, J. R. (2010). Training with mild anxiety may prevent choking under higher levels of anxiety. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 11, 44–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oudejans, R. R. D., Van de Langeberg, R. W., & Hutter, R. I. (2002). Aiming at a far target under different viewing conditions: Visual control in basketball jump shooting. Human Movement Science, 21, 457–480.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Payne, B. K. (2001). Prejudice and perception: The role of automatic and controlled processes in misperceiving a weapon. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 181–192.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pijpers, J. R., Oudejans, R. R. D., Bakker, F. C., & Beek, P. J. (2006). The role of anxiety in perceiving and realizing affordances. Ecological Psychology, 18, 131–161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pijpers, J. R., Oudejans, R. R. D., Holsheimer, F., & Bakker, F. C. (2003). Anxiety performance relationships in climbing: A process-oriented approach. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 4, 283–304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Proffitt, D. R. (2006). Embodied perception and the economy of action. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 110–122.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sarason, I. G. (1988). Anxiety, self-preoccupation and attention. Anxiety Research, 1, 3–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schutter, D., Hofman, D., & Van Honk, J. (2008). Fearful faces selectively increase corticospinal motor tract excitability: A transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Psychophysiology, 45, 345–348.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schwenkmezger, P., & Steffgen, G. (1989). Anxiety and motor performance. In B. Kirkcaldy (Ed.), Normalities and abnormalities in human movement (pp. 78–99). Basel: Karger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Somerville, L. H., Kim, H., Johnstone, T., Alexander, A. L., & Whalen, P. J. (2004). Human amygdala responses during presentations of happy and neutral faces: Correlations with state anxiety. Biological Psychiatry, 55, 897–903.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spence, J. T., & Spence, K. W. (1966). The motivational components of manifest anxiety: Drive and drive stimuli. In C. Spielberger (Ed.), Anxiety and behavior (pp. 291–326). New York: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Stins, J. F., Roelofs, K., Villan, J., Kooijman, K., Hagenaars, M. A., & Beek, P. J. (2011). Walk to me when I smile, step back when I’m angry: Emotional faces modulate whole-body approach–avoidance behaviors. Experimental Brain Research, 212, 603–611.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Teachman, B. A., Stefanucci, J. K., Clerkin, E. M., Cody, M. W., & Proffitt, D. R. (2008). A new mode of fear expression: Perceptual bias in height fear. Emotion, 8, 296–301.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Vickers, J. N. (2007). Perception, cognition and decision training: The quiet eye in action. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vickers, J. N., & Williams, A. M. (2007). Performing under pressure: The effects of physiological arousal, cognitive anxiety, and gaze control in biathlon. Journal of Motor Behavior, 39, 381–394.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wegner, D. M. (1994). Ironic processes of mental control. Psychological Review, 101, 34–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Whiting, H. T. A. (1984). The concepts of ‘adaptation’ and ‘attunement’ in skill learning. In M. A. Arbib, E. L. Rissland, & O. Selfridge (Eds.), Adaptive control of ill defined systems. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, A. M., Davids, K., & Williams, J. G. (1999). Visual perception and action in sport. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, M. (2002). Six views of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 625–636.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, M. R. (2008). From processing efficiency to attentional control: A mechanistic account of the anxiety-performance relationship. International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 1, 184–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, M. R., & Smith, N. C. (2007). A test of the predictions of processing efficiency theory during elite team competition using the thought occurrence questionnaire for sport. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 38, 245–262.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, M. R., Vine, S. J., Masters, R. S. W., McGrath, J. (2011). Gaze training enhances laparoscopic technical skill acquisition and multi-tasking performance: A randomized controlled study. Surgical Endoscopy (in press). doi:10.1007/s00464-011-1802-2.

  • Wilson, M. R., Vine, S. J., & Wood, G. (2009). The influence of anxiety on visual attentional control in basketball free throw shooting. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 31, 152–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, M. R., Wood, G., & Vine, S. J. (2009). Anxiety, attentional control, and performance impairment in penalty kicks. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 31, 761–775.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wine, J. (1971). Test anxiety and direction of attention. Psychological Bulletin, 76, 92–104.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Withagen, R., & Michaels, C. F. (2005). The role of feedback information for calibration and attunement in perceiving length by dynamic touch. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 1379–1390.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Witt, J. K., Proffitt, D. R., & Epstein, W. (2010). When and how are spatial perceptions scaled? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 1153–1160.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yerkes, R. M., & Dodson, J. D. (1908). The relationship of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit-formation. Journal of Comparative Neurology of Psychology, 18, 459–482.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yoshie, M., Kudo, M., Murakoshi, T., & Ohtsuki, T. (2009). Music performance anxiety in skilled pianists: Effects of social-evaluative performance situation on subjective, autonomic, and electromyographic reactions. Experimental Brain Research, 199, 117–126.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 35, 151–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Open Access

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Noncommercial License which permits any noncommercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author(s) and source are credited.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Arne Nieuwenhuys.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Nieuwenhuys, A., Oudejans, R.R.D. Anxiety and perceptual-motor performance: toward an integrated model of concepts, mechanisms, and processes. Psychological Research 76, 747–759 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-011-0384-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-011-0384-x

Keywords

Navigation