Skip to main content
Log in

The effects of verbal and spatial interference in the encoding and retrieval of spatial and nonspatial texts

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The paper investigates the specific roles of visual–spatial working memory (VSWM) and verbal working memory (VWM) in encoding and retrieval of information conveyed by spatial and nonspatial texts. In two experiments, a total of 109 undergraduate students—54 in Experiment 1, 55 in Experiment 2—listened to spatial and nonspatial texts while performing a spatial (Experiment 1) or verbal (Experiment 2) concurrent task during either encoding or retrieval. Text memorisation and comprehension were tested by free-recall and sentence-verification tasks. The results show that a concurrent spatial task is detrimental to memory performance for spatial text more than for nonspatial text. In contrast, a concurrent verbal task is equally damaging to memory performance for both spatial and nonspatial texts. Moreover, a spatial task interferes with both encoding and retrieval, in contrast with a verbal task, where the interference effect is active only when the task is performed during encoding. Overall, these findings show the involvement of VSWM in the construction and reactivation of mental models derived from spatial descriptions, and the role played by VWM in construction, but not reactivation, of mental models derived from spatial and nonspatial texts.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D. (1990). Human memory: Theory and practice. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D. (2003). Working memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 829–839.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D., Lewis, V. J., & Vallar, G. (1984). Exploring the articulatory loop. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 36, 233–252.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baguley, T., & Payne, S. J. (2000). Given new versus new-given? An analysis of reading time for spatial descriptions. In S. O. Nuallain (Ed.), Spatial cognition: Foundations and application: Selected papers from Mind III, Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society of Ireland, 1998. Advances in Consciousness Research (pp. 317–328). Amsterdam, Netherlands: John Benjamins Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bower, G. H., & Morrow, D. G. (1990). Mental models in narrative comprehension. Science, 247, 44–48.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brooks, L. R. (1967). The suppression of visualization by reading. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 19, 289–299.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daniel, M. P., & Denis, M. (2004). The production of route directions: Investigating conditions that favour conciseness in spatial discourse. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18, 57–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Daniel, M. P., Tom, A., Manghi, E., & Denis, M. (2003). Testing the value of route directions through navigational performance. Spatial Cognition and Computation, 3, 269–289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Beni, R., Pazzaglia, F., Gyselenck, V., & Meneghetti, C. (2005). Visuo-spatial working memory and mental representation of spatial descriptions. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 17 (1), 77–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Denis, M. (1996). Imagery and the description of spatial configurations. In M. de Vega, M. J. Intons-Peterson, P. M. Johnson-Laird, M. Denis, & M. Marschark (Eds.), Models of visual–spatial cognition (pp. 128–197). London, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Denis, M., & Cocude, M. (1997). On the metric properties of visual images generated from verbal descriptions: Evidence for the robustness of mental scanning effect. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9, 353–379.

    Google Scholar 

  • Denis, M., Daniel, M. P., Fontaine, S., & Pazzaglia, F. (2001). Language, spatial cognition, and navigation. In M. Denis, R. H. Logie, C. Cornoldi, M. de Vega, & J. Engelkamp (Eds.), Imagery, language and visuo-spatial thinking (pp.137–160). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Denis, M., & Denhière, G. (1990). Comprehension and recall of spatial descriptions. European Bulletin of Cognitive Psychology, 10, 115–143.

    Google Scholar 

  • Denis, M., Logie, R. H., Cornoldi, C., de Vega, M., & Engelkamp, J. (2001). Imagery, language and visuo-spatial thinking. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deyzac, E., Logie, R. H., & Denis, M. (2006). Visuo-spatial working memory and the processing of spatial descriptions. British Journal of Psychology (in press).

  • van Dijk, T. A., & Kintsch, W. (1983). Strategies of discourse comprehension. New York, NY: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garden, S., Cornoldi, C., & Logie, R. H. (2002). Visuo-spatial working memory in navigation. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 16, 35–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, S., & Baddeley, A. D. (1993). Working memory and language. Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gyselinck, V., Cornoldi, C., Dubois, V., De Beni, R., & Ehrlich, M. F. (2002). Visuo-spatial memory and phonological loop in learning from multimedia. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 16, 665–685.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gyselinck, V., Ehrlich, M. F., Cornoldi, C., De Beni, R., & Dubois, V. (2000). Visual–spatial working memory in learning from multimedia systems. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning, 16, 166–176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kintsch, W., & van Dijk, T. A. (1978). Toward a model of text comprehension and production. Psychological Review, 85, 363–394.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kruley, P., Sciama, S. C., & Glenberg, A. M. (1994). On-line processing of textual illustrations in the visual–spatial sketchpad: Evidence from dual-task studies. Memory and Cognition, 22, 261–272.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landau, B., & Jackendoff, R. (1993). “What” and “Where” in spatial language and spatial cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 217–265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levelt, W. J. M. (1989). Speaking: From intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levy, B. A. (1971). The role of articulation in auditory and visual short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 10, 123–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levy, B. A. (1975). Vocalization and suppression effects in sentence memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14, 304–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Logie, R. H. (1995). Visuo-spatial working memory, Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Logie, R. H., & Salway, A. F. S. (1990). Working memory and modes of thinking: A secondary task approach. In K. Gilhooly, M. Keane, R. Logie, & G. Erdos (Eds.), Lines of thinking: Reflections on the psychology of thought, Vol. 1 (pp. 99–13). Chichester, UK: Wiley.

  • McCarthy, R. A., & Warrington, E. K. (1987). The double dissociation of short-term memory for lists and sentences. Evidence from aphasia. Brain, 110, 1273–1296.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morrow, D. G., Bower, G. H., & Greenspan, S. L. (1989). Situation-based inferences during narrative comprehension. In A.C. Graesser, & G. H. Bower (Eds.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Inferences and text comprehension (pp. 123–136). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morrow, D. G., Greenspan, S. L., & Bower, G. H. (1987). Accessibility and situational models in narrative comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 26, 165–187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Noordzij, M. L., van der Lubbe, R. H. J., Neggers, S. F. W., & Postma, A. (2004). Spatial tapping interferes with the processing of linguistic spatial relations, Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 259–271.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pazzaglia, F., & Cornoldi, C. (1999). The role of distinct components of visuo-spatial working memory in the processing of texts. Memory, 7, 19–41.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Perrig, W., & Kintsch, W. (1985). Propositional and situational representations of text. Journal of Memory and Language, 24, 503–518.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Quinn, J. G., & Ralston, G. E. (1986). Movement and attention in visual working memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38A, 689–703.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smyth, M. M., & Pendleton, L. R. (1989). Working memory for movements. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41A, 235–250.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, H. A., & Tversky, B. (1992). Spatial mental models derived from survey and route descriptions. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 261–292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vallar, G., & Baddeley, A. D. (1984). Fractionation of working memory: Neuropsychological evidence for a phonological short-term store. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 23, 151–161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vallar, G., & Baddeley, A. D. (1987). Phonological short-term store and sentence processing. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 4, 417–438.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Vega, M. (1995). Backward updating of mental models during continuous reading of narrative. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 21, 373–385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Vega, M., Cocude, M., Denis, M., Rodrigo, M. J., & Zimmer, H. D. (2001). The interface between language and visuo-spatial representation. In M. Denis, R. H. Logie, C. Cornoldi, M. de Vega, & J. Engelkamp (Eds.), Imagery, language and visuo-spatial thinking (pp. 137–160). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors wish to thank Alessandra Canossa for collecting the Experiment 2 data, and are also grateful to Valerie Gyselinck for valuable suggestions.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Francesca Pazzaglia.

Additional information

Support for this research was provided by a PRIN 2003 grant to the first author. A preliminary report of this experiment was presented at the European Workshop of Imagery and Cognition (2003).

Appendix 1

Appendix 1

Spatial text

Imagine yourself standing in a meadow dotted with flowers in front of the tall boundary walls of a holiday farm called “Tranquillity” stretching over a rectangular area.

On your right, on the corner of the holiday farm, there is the entrance gate...

...Once you have reached the restaurant, turn left and, leaving the restaurant behind, you will soon pass a little bridge crossing a small lake...

...When you get there, turn left leaving the barn behind you and continue on your way for a while. You will see the poultry pen on your right...

...Turn left and continue walking for a long way along the brick wall; keeping the vineyard on the left. You will then get back to the gate.

Nonspatial text

The grape harvest is performed at different times of the year depending on the quality of the wine: sparkling wine during the first week of September, and non-sparkling wine during the second week of September...

...Non-sparkling wines are obtained from fully matured grapes and have a high alcohol level and low acidity...

...There are two types of vinification process, i.e. two different ways to make wine, for red and white wine.

...Before bottling, crystallisation takes place by bringing the wine to sub-zero temperatures, about −5°C. This procedure lasts 2 days and allows the excess tartar to deposit so it can be eliminated later.

To produce white wines, the grapes are crushed and the skins immediately discarded: the must obtained is put into casks.

Fermentation occurs at 15–18°C, then crystallisation and bottling takes place.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Pazzaglia, F., De Beni, R. & Meneghetti, C. The effects of verbal and spatial interference in the encoding and retrieval of spatial and nonspatial texts. Psychological Research 71, 484–494 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-006-0045-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-006-0045-7

Keywords

Navigation