Psychological Research

, Volume 70, Issue 2, pp 77–87 | Cite as

Perceived duration of expected and unexpected stimuli

Original Article

Abstract

Three experiments assessed whether perceived stimulus duration depends on whether participants process an expected or an unexpected visual stimulus. Participants compared the duration of a constant standard stimulus with a variable comparison stimulus. Changes in expectancy were induced by presenting one type of comparison more frequently than another type. Experiment 1 used standard durations of 100 and 400 ms, and Experiments 2 and 3 durations of 400 and 800 ms. Stimulus frequency did not affect perceived duration in Experiment 1. In Experiments 2 and 3, however, frequent comparisons were perceived as shorter than infrequent ones, and discrimination performance was better for infrequent comparisons. Overall, this study supports the notion that infrequent stimuli increase the speed of an internal pacemaker.

References

  1. Allan, L. G., Kristofferson, A. B., & Wiens, E. W. (1971). Duration discrimination of brief light flashes. Perception and Psychophysics, 9, 327–334.Google Scholar
  2. Block, R. A. (1994). Time-order errors. In S. L. Macey (Ed.), Encyclopedia of time (pp. 632). New York: Garland.Google Scholar
  3. Brown, S. W. (1995). Time perception and attention: The effects of practice versus retrospective paradigms and task demands on perceived duration. Perception & Psychophysics, 38, 115–124.Google Scholar
  4. Brown, S. W. (1997). Attention resources in timing: Interference effects in concurrent temporal and nontemporal working memory tasks. Perception & Psychophysics, 59, 1118–1140.Google Scholar
  5. Brown, S. W., & Boltz, M. G. (2002). Attentional processes in time perception: Effects of mental workload and event structure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28, 600–615.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Bundesen, C. (1990). A theory of visual attention. Psychological Review, 97, 523–547.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Busey, T. A., & Loftus, G. R. (1994). Sensory and cognitive components of visual information acquisition. Psychological Review, 101, 446–469.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Bush, R. R. (1963). Estimation and evaluation. In R. D. Luce, R. R. Bush, & E. Galanter (Eds.), Handbook of mathematical psychology: Vol. 1 (pp. 429–469). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  9. Casini, L., & Macar, F. (1999). Multiple approaches to investigate the existence of an internal clock using attentional resources. Behavioural Processes, 45, 73–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Chen, Z., & O’Neill, P. (2001). Processing demand modulates the effects of spatial attention on the judged duration of brief stimulus. Perception & Psychophysics, 63, 1229–1238.Google Scholar
  11. Church, R. M., & Gibbon, J. (1982). Temporal generalization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 8, 165–186.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Creelman, C. D. (1962). Human discrimination of auditory stimuli. Journal of the Acoustical Society, 34, 582–593.Google Scholar
  13. Droit-Volet, S. (2002). Scalar timing in temporal generalization in children with short and long stimulus durations. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 55A, 1193–1209.Google Scholar
  14. Efron, R. (1970). The relationship between the duration of a stimulus and the duration of a perception. Neuropsychologia, 8, 37–55.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Enns, J. T., Brehaut, J. C., & Shore, D. I. (1999). The duration of a brief event in the mind’s eye. Journal of General Psychology, 126, 355–372.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Fortin, C. (2003). Attentional time-sharing in interval timing. In W. H. Meck (Ed.), Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing (pp. 235–260). Boca Raton, FL: CRC.Google Scholar
  17. Getty, D. J. (1975). Discrimination of short temporal intervals: A comparison of two models. Perception & Psychophysics, 18, 1–8.Google Scholar
  18. Gibbon, J. (1977). Scalar expectancy theory and Weber’s law in animal timing. Psychological Review, 84, 279–325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Gibbon, J. (1991). Origins of scalar timing. Learning and Motivation, 22, 3–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Goldstone, S., & Goldfarb, J. L. (1964). Auditory and visual time judgment. Journal of General Psychology, 70, 369–387.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Goldstone, S., & Lhamon, W. T. (1974). Studies of auditory-visual differences in human time judgment. I. Sounds are judges longer than lights. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 39, 63–82.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Goldstone, S., Lhamon, W. T., & Sechzer, J. (1978). Light intensity and judged duration. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 12, 83–84.Google Scholar
  23. Goodfellow, L. D. (1934). An empirical comparison of audition, vision, and touch in the discrimination of short intervals of time. American Journal of Psychology, 46, 243–258.Google Scholar
  24. Grondin, S. (1993). Duration discrimination of empty and filled intervals marked by auditory and visual signals. Perception & Psychophysics, 54, 383–394.Google Scholar
  25. Grondin, S. (2001a). From physical time to the first and second moments of psychological time. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 22–44.Google Scholar
  26. Grondin, S. (2001b). Discriminating time intervals presented in sequences marked by visual signals. Perception & Psychophysics, 63, 1214–1228.Google Scholar
  27. Hemmes, N. S., Brown, B. L., & Kladopoulos, C. N. (2004). Time perception with and without a concurrent nontemporal task. Perception & Psychophysics, 66, 328–341.Google Scholar
  28. Jacoby, L. L., & Dallas, M. (1981). On the relationship between autobiographical memory and perceptual learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 110, 306–340.Google Scholar
  29. Kaernbach, C. (1991). Simple adaptive testing with the weighted up-down method. Perception & Psychophysics, 49, 227–229.Google Scholar
  30. Killeen, P. R., & Fetterman, J. G. (1988). A behavioral theory of timing. Psychological Review, 95, 274–295.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Lejeune, H. (1998). Switching or gating? The attentional challenge in cognitive models of psychological time. Behavioural Processes, 44, 127–145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Lewis, P. A., & Miall, R. C. (2003). Distinct systems for automatic and cognitively controlled time measurement: Evidence from neuroimaging. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 13, 1–6.Google Scholar
  33. Loftus, G. R., & Masson, M. E. J. (1994). Using confidence intervals in within-subjects designs. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1, 476–490.Google Scholar
  34. Malapani, C., & Fairhurst, S. (2002). Scalar timing in animals and humans. Learning and Motivation, 33, 156–176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Mattes, S., & Ulrich, R. (1998). Directed attention prolongs the perceived duration of a brief stimulus. Perception & Psychophysics, 60, 1305–1317.Google Scholar
  36. McCormack, T., Brown, G. D. A., Maylor, E. A., Richardson, L. B. N., & Darby, R. J. (2002). Effects of aging on absolute identification of duration. Psychology and Aging, 17, 363–378.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Michon, J. A. (1985). The compleat time experiencer. In J. A. Michon & J. L. Jackson (Eds.), Time, mind, and behavior (pp. 21–52). Berlin Heidelberg New York: Springer.Google Scholar
  38. Miller, J., Franz, V., & Ulrich, R. (1999). Effects of auditory stimulus intensity on response force in simple, go/no-go, and choice RT tasks. Perception & Psychophysics, 61, 107–119.Google Scholar
  39. Pachella, R. G. (1975). The effect of set on the tachistoscopic recognition of pictures. In P. M. A. Rabbitt and S. Dornic (Eds.), Attention and performance V (pp. 136–156). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  40. Pang, K., Merkel, F., Egeth, H., & Olton, D. S. (1992). Expectancy and stimulus frequency: A comparative analysis in rats and humans. Perception & Psychophysics, 51, 607–615.Google Scholar
  41. Pashler, H. E. (1998). The psychology of attention. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  42. Penney, T. B., Gibbon, J., & Meck, W. H. (2000). Differential effects of auditory and visual signals on clock speed and temporal memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 1770–1787.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Penton-Voak, I. S., Edwards, H., Percival, A., & Wearden, J. H. (1996). Speeding up an internal clock in humans? Effects of click trains on subjective duration. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 22, 307–320.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Posner, M. I. (1978). Chronometric explorations of mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  45. Posner, M. I., Nissen, M. J., & Ogden, W. C. (1978). Attended and unattended processing modes: The role of set for spatial location. In H. L. Pick, Jr., & I. J. Saltzman (Eds.), Modes of perceiving and processing information (pp. 137–157). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  46. Postman, L., & Miller, G. A. (1945). Anchoring of temporal judgements. American Journal of Psychology, 58, 43–53.Google Scholar
  47. Rammsayer, T. (1996). Experimental evidence for different timing mechanisms underlying temporal discrimination. In S. Masin (Ed.), Fechner Day 96. Twelfth annual meeting of the International Society for Psychophysics (pp. 63–68). Padua, Italy: The International Society for Psychophysics.Google Scholar
  48. Rammsayer, T. (1999). Neuropharmacological evidence for different timing mechanism in humans. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 52B, 273–286.Google Scholar
  49. Rammsayer, T. (2003). Sensory and cognitive mechanisms in temporal processing elucidated by a model systems approach. In H. Helfrich (Ed.), Time and mind II: Information processing perspectives (pp. 97–113). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe & Huber.Google Scholar
  50. Rammsayer, T. H., & Lima, S. D. (1991). Duration discrimination of filled and empty auditory intervals: Cognitive and perceptual factors. Perception & Psychophysics, 50, 565–574.Google Scholar
  51. Rammsayer, T., & Ulrich, R. (2001). Counting models of temporal discrimination. Psychomomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 270–277.Google Scholar
  52. Reinitz, M. T. (1990). Effects of spatially directed attention on visual encoding. Perception & Psychophysics, 47, 497–505.Google Scholar
  53. Sanders, A. F. (1998). Elements of human performance: Reaction processes and attention in human skill. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  54. Sokolov, E. N. (1963). Perception and the conditioned reflex. Oxford: Pergamon.Google Scholar
  55. Stelmach, L. B., & Herdman, C. M. (1991). Directed attention and the perception of temporal order. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 17, 539–550.Google Scholar
  56. Thomas, E. A. C., & Weaver, W. B. (1975). Cognitive processing in time perception. Perception & Psychophysics, 17, 363–367.Google Scholar
  57. Treisman, M. (1963). Temporal discrimination and the indifference interval: Implications for a model of the “internal clock.” Psychological Monographs, 77, 1–31.Google Scholar
  58. Treisman, M., Faulkner, A., Naish, P. L., & Brogan, D. (1990). The internal clock: Evidence for a temporal oscillator underlying time perception with some estimates of its characteristic frequency. Perception, 19, 705–743.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  59. Treisman, M., Faulkner, A., & Naish, P. L. (1992). On the relation between time perception and the timing of motor action: Evidence for a temporal oscillator controlling the timing of movement. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Experimental Psychology, 45a, 235–263.Google Scholar
  60. Ulrich, R., & Mattes, S. (1996). Does immediate arousal enhance response force in simple reaction time? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A, 972–990.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Walker, J. T., & Scott, K. J. (1981). Auditory-visual conflicts in the perceived duration of lights, tones, and gaps. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, 1327–1339.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  62. Wearden, J. H. (1991). Human performance as an analogue of an interval bisection task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 43B, 59–81.Google Scholar
  63. Wearden, J. H. (1995). Categorical scaling of stimulus duration by humans. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 21, 318–330.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  64. Wearden, J. H., Edwards, H., Fakhri, M., & Percival, A. (1998). Why “sounds are judged longer than lights”: Application of a model of the internal clock in humans. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 51B, 97–120.Google Scholar
  65. Witherspoon, D., & Allan, L. G. (1985). The effect of a prior presentation on temporal judgments in a perceptual identification task. Memory & Cognition, 13, 101–111.Google Scholar
  66. Zakay, D., & Block, R. A. (1997). Temporal cognition. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 6, 12–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2004

Authors and Affiliations

  • Rolf Ulrich
    • 1
  • Judith Nitschke
    • 1
  • Thomas Rammsayer
    • 2
  1. 1.Psychologisches InstitutUniversität TübingenTübingenGermany
  2. 2.Universität GöttingenGöttingenGermany

Personalised recommendations