Skip to main content
Log in

Serial position effects in implicit memory for multiple-digit numbers

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Serial position effects in implicit and explicit memory were investigated in a short-term memory task. A study list composed of four, spatially distributed, two-digit numbers was presented, followed by an item recognition task (explicit test) and an implicit memory task in which participants were asked to verify a simple addition equation where the presented answer was either primed or not primed by one of the number pairs in the study list. Similar serial position effects were observed in explicit and implicit memory, with faster response times for correct decisions on the first than on the later list positions. The presence of a primacy effect but no recency effect is consistent with previous studies of explicit memory with visual presentation. The results suggest that similar principles of temporal information processing govern priming and episodic short-term memory.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Arbuthnott, K. D. (1996). To repeat or not to repeat: Repetition facilitation and inhibition in sequential retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 125, 261–283.

    Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, R. C., & Shiffrin R. M. (1968). Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. In K. W. Spence & J. T. Spence (Eds.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol.2, pp. 89–195). San Diego: Academic.

  • Brooks, B. M. (1994). A comparison of serial position effects in implicit and explicit word-stem completion. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 1, 264–268.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brooks, B. M. (1999). Primacy and recency in primed free association and associative cued recall. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 6, 479–485.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, J. I. D. (1991). Conditions of error priming in number fact retrieval. Memory and Cognition, 19, 197–209.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell J. I. D., & Tarling, D. P. M. (1996). Production, verification and error priming in cognitive arithmetic. Memory and Cognition, 24, 156–172.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conrad, R., & Hull, A. J. (1968). Input modality and the serial position curve in short-term memory. Psychonomic Science, 10, 135–136.

    Google Scholar 

  • Craik, F. I. M. (2003). Commentary. In J. S. Bowers & C. J. Marsolek (Eds.), Rethinking implicit memory (pp. 327–336). New York: Oxford University Press.

  • Craik, F. I. M., & Lockhart, R. S. (1972). Levels of processing: A framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 671–684.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crowder, R. G. (1993). Short-term memory: Where do we stand? Memory and Cognition, 21, 142–145.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Delazer, M., & Girelli, L. (2000). Priming arithmetic reasoning in an amnesic patient. Brain and Cognition, 43, 138–143.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Delazer, M., Ewen, P., & Benke, T. (1997). Priming arithmetic facts in amnesic patients. Neuropsychologia, 35, 623–634.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Foster, J. K., & Jelicic, M. (1999). Memory: Systems, process or function? New York: Oxford University Press.

  • Frick, R. W. (1984). Using both an auditory and a visual short-term store to increase digit span. Memory and Cognition, 12, 507–514.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gershberg, F. B., & Shimamura, A. P. (1994). Serial position effects in implicit and explicit tests of memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 20, 1370–1378.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg, A. M., Bradley, M. M., Stevenson, J. A., Kraus, T. A., Tkachuk, M. J., Gretz, A. L., Fish, J. H. & Turp, B. M. (1980). A two-process account of long-term serial position effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6, 355–369.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Helstrup, T, & Magnussen, S. (2001). The mental representation of familiar, long-distance journeys. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 21, 411–421.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jacoby, L. L., & Dallas, M. (1981). On the relationship between autobiographical memory and perceptual learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 110, 306–340.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kuster, J. P. (1998). Serial position effects in the implicit memory task of word stem completion. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering, 59, 1396.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lamberts, K., Brockdorff, N., & Heit, E. (2003). Feature-sampling and random walk models of individual-stimulus recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132, 351–378.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LeCompte, D. C. (1992). In search of strong visual recency effects. Memory and Cognition, 20, 263–272.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lively, B. L., & Sanford, B. J. (1972). The use of category information in a memory-search task. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 93, 379–385.

    Google Scholar 

  • Magnussen, S. (2000). Low-level memory processes in vision. Trends in Neurosciences, 23, 247–251.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Magnussen, S., Idås, E., & Holst Myhre, S. (1998). Representation of orientation and spatial frequency in perception and memory: A choice reaction time analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human perception and performance, 24, 707–718.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maylor, E. A. (2002). Serial position effects in semantic memory: Reconstructing the order of verses of hymns. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9, 816–820.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McDermott, K. B. (2000). Implicit memory. In A. E. Kazdin (Ed.), The encyclopedia of psychology (pp. 231–234). New York: Oxford University Press.

  • McKenzie, W. A., & Humphreys, M. S. (1991). Recency effects in direct and indirect memory tasks. Memory and Cognition, 19, 321–331.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Nairne, J. S. (1991). Positional uncertainty in long-term memory. Memory and Cognition, 19, 332–340.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Nordby, K., Raanaas, R. K., & Magnussen, S. (2002). The expanding telephone number. I. Keying briefly presented multiple-digit numbers. Behaviour and Information Technology, 21, 27–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Phaf, R. H., & Wolters, G. (1996). Elaboration effects in implicit and explicit memory tests. Psychological Research, 58, 284–293.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinto, A. D. C., & Baddeley, A. D. (1991). Where did you park your car? Analysis of naturalistic long-term recency effect. European Journal of Psychology, 3, 297–313.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poldrack, R. A., Desmond, J. E., Glover, G. H. & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (1998). The neural basis of visual skill learning: An fMRI study of mirror reading. Cerebral Cortex, 8, 1–10.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raanaas, R. K., & Magnussen, S. Serial position effects in implicit memory. Manuscript submitted for publication.

  • Raanaas, R. K., Nordby, K., & Magnussen, S. (2002). The expanding telephone number. II. Age variations in immediate memory for multiple-digit numbers. Behaviour and Information Technology, 21, 39–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds, J. H., & Goldstein, J. A. (1974). The effects of category membership on memory scanning for words. American Journal of Psychology, 87, 487–495.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L. III. (2003). Reconsidering implicit memory. In J. S. Bower & C. J. Marsolek (Eds.), Rethinking implicit memory (pp. 3–18). New York: Oxford University Press.

  • Roediger, H. L. III., & Crowder, R. G. (1976). A serial position effect in recall of United States presidents. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 8, 275–278.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L. III., Buckner, R., & McDermott, K. B. (1999). Components of processing. In J. K. Foster & M. Jelicic (Eds.), Memory: Systems, process, or function? (pp 31–65). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rybash, J. M., & Osborne, J. L. (1991). Implicit memory, the serial position effect, and test awareness. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 29, 327–330.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sloman, S. A., Hayman, C. A. G., Otha, N., Law, J., & Tulving, E. (1988). Forgetting in primed fragment completion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 14, 223–239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E. (1999). Study of memory: process and systems. In J. K. Foster, & M. Jelicic (Eds.), Memory: Systems, process or function? (pp. 11–30). New York: Oxford University Press.

  • Tulving, E., & Schacter, D. L. (1990). Priming and human memory systems. Science, 247, 301–306.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Verfaellie, M., Gabrieli, J. D. E., Vaidya, C. J., & Croce, P. (1996). Implicit memory for pictures in amnesia: Role of etiology and priming task. Neuropsychology, 10, 517–537.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This study was supported by the Norwegian Research Council (MH). We thank David Geary and Irving Koch for constructive comments on a previous version of the paper.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ruth K. Raanaas.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Raanaas, R.K., Magnussen, S. Serial position effects in implicit memory for multiple-digit numbers. Psychological Research 70, 26–31 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-004-0185-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-004-0185-6

Keywords

Navigation