Skip to main content
Log in

Spatial interference and response control in sequence learning: the role of explicit knowledge

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In several sequence learning studies it has been suggested that response control shifts from the stimuli to some internal representation (i.e., motor program) through the learning process. The main questions addressed in this paper are whether this control shift is related to explicit knowledge and whether the formation of these internal representations depends on the stimulus attributes. In one experiment we compared the learning of a response sequence triggered by either spatial location or location symbol (left-right) by using a serial response task (SRT). Symbols were presented at either a centered or random location. The results showed that in the symbolic conditions the shift of response control correlated with the emergence of explicit knowledge. Only participants with complete explicit knowledge seemed to learn the sequence structure beyond probabilistic information (response time "RT" did not depend on the frequency of the response). Moreover, these participants were able to overcome, when needed, spatial interference (RT was the same for both spatially corresponding and non-corresponding trials). However, when spatial location was relevant, RT was always faster, especially for more frequent responses. These results suggest that the relevant stimulus dimension (location or symbol) seems to engage different sequence learning mechanisms.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1.
Fig. 2.
Fig. 3.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. We thank Iring Koch for his suggestion for similar analyses involving the sequence structure.

  2. A pilot study showed that explicit learning was very difficult to obtain in this condition even with intentional instructions (only one-third of the participants were able to write the complete sequence).

  3. This effect was only marginal in the first random block (p=0.08), probably due to a longer RSI (250 ms) than that observed for the response repetition effect (50 ms).

References

  • Baker, J.T., Donoghue, J.P., & Sanes, J.N. (1999). Gaze direction modulates finger movement activation patterns in humans. Journal of Neuroscience, 15, 10044–10052.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bertelson, P. (1965). Serial choice reaction time as a function of response versus signal and response repetition. Nature, 205, 217–218.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bower, G. H., & Winzenz, D. (1969). Group structure, coding, and memory for digit sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology Monograph Supplement, 80, 1–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Jong, R., Liang, C.C., & Lauber, E. (1994). Conditional and unconditional automaticity: A dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 731–750.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eimer, M., Hommel, B., & Prinz, W. (1995). S-R compatibility and response selection. Acta Psychologica, 90, 301–313.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eliassen, J.C., Souza, T., & Sanes, J.N. (2001). Human brain activation accompanying explicitly directed movement sequence learning. Experimental Brain Research, 141, 269–280.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Frensch, P. A. (1994). Composition during serial learning: a serial position effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 20, 423–442.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Frensch, P. A., & Miner, C. S. (1994). Effects of presentation rate and individual differences in short-term memory capacity on an indirect measure of serial learning. Memory and Cognition, 22, 95–110.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hazeltine, E., Grafton, S. T., & Ivry, R. (1997). Attention and stimulus characteristics determine the locus of motor-sequence encoding. A PET study. Brain, 120, 123–140.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Helmuth, L. L., Mayr, U., & Daum, I (2000). Sequence learning in Parkinson's disease: a comparison of spatial attention and number-response sequences. Neuropsychologia, 38, 1143–1451.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoffmann, J., & Koch, I. (1997). Stimulus-response compatibility and sequential learning in the serial reaction time task. Psychological Research, 60, 87–97.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffmann, J., & Koch, I. (1998). Implicit learning of loosely defined structures. In M. A. Stadler, & P. A. Frensch (Eds.). Handbook of implicit learning. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

  • Hommel, B. (2003). Acquisition and control of voluntary action. In S. Maasen, W. Prinz, & G. Roth (Eds.) Voluntary action: Brains, minds, and sociality (pp. 34–48). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koch, I., & Hoffmann, J. (2000a). The role of stimulus-based and response-based spatial information in sequence learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 863–882.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Koch, I., & Hoffmann, J. (2000b). Patterns, chunks, and hierarchies in serial reaction-time tasks. Psychological Research, 63, 22–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lashley, K. S. (1951). The problem of serial order in behavior. In L. A. Jeffress (Ed.), Cerebral Mechanisms in Behavior. The Hixon Symposium (pp. 112–136). New York: Wiley.

  • Lawrence, B. M., Myerson, J., Oonk, H. M., & Abrams, R. A. (2001). The effects of eye and limb movements on working memory. Memory, 9, 433–444.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Logie, R. H. (1995). Visuo-spatial working memory. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

  • Lu, C. H., & Proctor, R.W. (1995). The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: a review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, 174–207.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayr, U. (1996). Spatial attention and implicit sequence learning. Evidence for independent learning of spatial and non-spatial sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 22, 350–364.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Nattkemper, D., & Prinz, W. (1997). Stimulus and response anticipation in a serial reaction task. Psychological Research, 60, 98–112.

    Google Scholar 

  • Notebaert, W., Soetens, E., & Melis, A. (2001). Sequential analysis of a Simon task—evidence for an attention-shift account. Psychological Research, 65, 170–184.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I. (1992). Attention as a cognitive and neural system. Psychological Science, 1, 11–14.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. (1992). Attentional mechanisms and consciousness experience. In A. D. Milner, & M. D. Rugg (Eds.), The Neuropsychology of Consciousness (pp. 91–111). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

  • Proctor, R. W., Wang, H., & Kim-Phoung L. (2002). Influences of different combinations of conceptual, perceptual, and structural similarity on stimulus-response compatibility. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55A, 59–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum, D. A., Kenny, S. B., & Derr, M. A. (1983). Hierarchical control of rapid movement sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9, 86–102.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simon, J. R., & Small, A.M., Jr. (1969). Processing auditory information: Interference from an irrelevant cue. Journal of Applied Psychology, 53, 433–435.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Willingham, D. B. (1998). A neuropsychological theory of motor skill learning. Psychological Review, 105, 558–584.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Willingham, D. B., Nissen, M. J., & Bullemer, P. (1989). On the development of procedural knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 15, 1047–1060.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Zirngibl, C., & Koch, I. (2002). The impact of response mode on implicit and explicit sequence learning. Experimental Psychology, 49, 153–162.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank Herbert Heuer, Iring Koch, Ulrich Mayr, and Bernhard Hommel for their helpful suggestions on a previous draft of this paper. We also thank Ivan Rodríguez for his assistance in running the experiment and Chris Chizk for checking the English. This research has been supported by the Ministerio de Ciencia y Tecnología of the Spanish Government (BSO2001-2008 and BSO2001-3264).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Elisabet Tubau.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Tubau, E., López-Moliner, J. Spatial interference and response control in sequence learning: the role of explicit knowledge. Psychological Research 68, 55–63 (2004). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-003-0139-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-003-0139-4

Keywords

Navigation