Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

The relationship between word length and threshold character size in patients with central scotoma and eccentric fixation

  • Clinical Investigation
  • Published:
Graefe's Archive for Clinical and Experimental Ophthalmology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Background

Understanding limitations on text reading with eccentric fixation is of major concern in low vision research. Our objective was to determine, in patients with a central scotoma, whether threshold character size is similar for different word lengths and paragraphed texts.

Methods

In 19 patients, we retrospectively analyzed the relationship between minimum readable character size for isolated words and text. Isolated letters, two, five, and ten-letter words and a paragraphed text were presented randomly through a scanning laser ophthalmoscope in eight different character sizes.

Results

Threshold character size varied according to the text stimulus (p<0.05). Threshold character sizes for single letters and two-letter words were matched (p>0.99), as were those for five-letter words, ten-letter words, and paragraphed text (p>0.99). Threshold character size for single letters and two-letter words was significantly lower than that measured with other text stimuli.

Discussion

Reading performance is influenced by a variety of factors such as crowding, contextual effects, visual span, degree of oculomotor adaptation needed, and frequency of a defined word. Globally, when reading with a central scotoma, it appears that within word characteristics have more impact than inter-word parameters on threshold character size.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Aquilante K, Yager D, Morris RA (2000) Can visual acuity predict the size of text that low vision readers need to read at maximum rates. In: Stuen C, Arditi A, Horowitz A, Lang MA, Rosenthal B, Seidman K (eds) Vision rehabilitation: assessment, intervention and outcomes. Swets and Zeitlinger, Lisse, pp 288–292

    Google Scholar 

  2. Arditi A (1994) On the relationship between letter acuity and reading acuity. In: Kooijman AC, Looijestijn PL, Welling JA, van der Wildt GJ (eds) Low vision research and new developments in rehabilitation. IOS, Amsterdam, pp 38–45

    Google Scholar 

  3. Bailey IL, Lovie JE (1980) The design and use of a new near-vision chart. Am J Optom Physiol Opt 57:378–387

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Baldasare J, Watson GR, Whittaker SG, Miller-Shaffer H (1986) The development and evaluation of a reading test for low vision individuals with macular loss. J Vis Impair Blind 80:785–789

    Google Scholar 

  5. Bouma H (1970) Interaction effects in parafoveal letter recognition. Nature 226:177–178

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Bullimore MA, Bailey IL (1995) Reading and eye movements in age-related maculopathy. Optom Vis Sci 72:125–138

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Cheong AMY, Lovie-Kitchin JE, Bowers AR (2002) Determining magnification for reading with low-vision. Optometry 85:229–237

    Google Scholar 

  8. Chung ST (2002) The effect of letter spacing on reading speed in central and peripheral vision. Investig Ophthalmol Vis Sci 43:1270–1276

    Google Scholar 

  9. Chung STL (2004) Reading speed benefits from increased vertical word spacing in normal peripheral vision. Optom Vis Sci 81:525–535

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Crossland MD, Culham LE, Rubin GS (2004) Fixation stability and reading in patients with newly developed macular disease. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt 24:327–333

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Cummings RW, Whittaker SG, Watson GR, Budd JM (1985) Scanning characters and reading with a central scotoma. Am J Optom Physiol Opt 62:833–843

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  12. Duret F, Buquet C, Charlier J, Viviani P, Safran AB (1999) Refixation strategies in four patients with macular disorders. Neuroophthalmology 22:209–222

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Eliott DB, Patel B, Whitaker D (2001) Development of a reading speed test for potential-vision measurements. Investig Ophthalmol Vis Sci 42 :1945–1949

    Google Scholar 

  14. Ergun E, Maar N, Radner W, Barbazetto I, Schmidt-Erfurth U, Stur M (2003) Scotoma size and reading speed in patients with subfoveal occult choroidal neovascularization in age-related macular degeneration. Ophthalmology 110:65–69

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Fine EM, Peli E (1996) The role of context in reading with central field loss. Optom Vis Sci 73:533–539

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  16. Fine EM, Rubin GS (1999) Reading with central field loss: number of letters masked is more important than the size of the mask in degrees. Vis Res 39:747–756

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  17. Fine EM, Hazel CA, Petre KL, Rubin GS (1999) Are the benefits of sentence context different in central and peripheral vision? Optom Vis Sci 76:764–769

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  18. Flom MC, Weymouth W, Kahneman D (1963) Visual resolution and contour interaction. J Opt Soc Am 55:1026–1032

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Heinen SJ, Skavensli AA (1992) Adaptation of saccades and fixation to bilateral foveal lesions in adult monkey. Vis Res 32:365–373

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  20. Higgins KE, Bailey IL (2000) Visual disorders and performance of specific tasks requiring vision. In: Silverstone B, Lang MA, Rosenthal B, Faye EE (eds) The lighthouse handbook of vision impairment and vision rehabilitation, Oxford University Press, New York, pp 287–315

    Google Scholar 

  21. Jacobs RJ (1979) Visual resolution and contour interaction on the fovea and periphery. Vis Res 19:1187–1195

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  22. Klitz TS, Legge GE, Tjan BS (2000) Saccade planning in reading with central scotomas: comparison of human and ideal performance. In: Kennedy A, Radach R, Heller D, Pynte J (eds) Reading as a perceptual process. Elsevier, New York, pp 667–682

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  23. Latham K, Whitaker D (1996) Relative roles of resolution and spatial interference in foveal and peripheral vision. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt 16:49–57

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  24. Leat SJ, Li W, Epp K (1999) Crowding in central and eccentric vision: the effects of contour interaction and attention. Investig Ophthalmol Vis Sci 40:504–512

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  25. Legge GE, Rubin GS, Pelli DG, Schleske MM (1985) Psychophysics of reading. II. Low vision. Vis Res 25:253–265

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  26. Legge GE, Ross JA, Luebker A, La May JM (1989) Psychophysics of reading. VIII. The Minnesota low-vision reading test. Optom Vis Sci 66:843–853

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  27. Legge GE, Ross JA, Maxwell KT, Luebker A (1989) Psychophysics of reading. VIII. Comprehension in normal and low vision. Clin Vis Sci 4:51–60

    Google Scholar 

  28. Legge GE, Ross JA, Isenberg LM, Lamay JM (1992) Psychophysics of reading: clinical predictors of low-vision reading speed. Investig Ophthalmol Vis Sci 33:677–687

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  29. Legge GE, Mansfield JS, Chung STL (2001) Psychophysics of reading. XX. Linking letter recognition to reading speed in central and peripheral vision. Vis Res 41:725–743

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  30. Legge GE, Hooven TA, Klitz TS, Stephen Mansfield JS, Tjan BS (2002) Mr. Chips 2002: new insights from an ideal-observer model of reading. Vis Res 42:2219–2234

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Levi DM, Klein SA, Harihara S (2002) Suppressive and facilitatory spatial interactions in foveal vision: foveal crowding is simple contrast masking. J Vis 2:46–65

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Liu L, Arditi A (2000) Apparent string shortening concomitant with letter crowding. Vis Res 40:1059–1067

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  33. Liu L, Arditi A (2001) How crowding affects letter confusion. Optom Vis Sci 78:50–55

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  34. O’Regan JK (1990) Eye movement and reading. In: Kowler E (ed) Eye movements and their role in visual and cognitive processes. Elsevier, New York, pp 395–453

    Google Scholar 

  35. Radner W, Willinger U, Obermayer W, Mudrich C, Velikay-Parel M, Eisenwort B (1998) Eine neue Lesentafel zur gleichzeitigen Bestimmung von Lesenvisus und Lesegeschmindigkeit. Klin Monatsbl Augenheilkd 213:174–181

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  36. Radner W, Obermayer W, Richter-Mueksch S, Willinger U, Velikay-Parel M, Eisenwort B (2002) The validity and reliability of short German sentences for measuring reading speed. Graefes Arch Clin Exp Ophthalmol 240:461–467

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Rayner K (1994) Eye movements during skilled reading. In: Ygge Y, Lennerstrand G (eds) Eye movements in reading. Elsevier, Oxford, pp 205–218

    Google Scholar 

  38. Rayner K, McConkie GW (1976) What guides a reader’s eye movements? Vis Res 16:829–837

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  39. Richter-Müksch S, Stur M, Stifter E, Kiss C, Velikay-Parel M, Radner W (2003) Different reading ability but same distance acuity of patients with drusen maculopathy and CNV-scars. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 44:[abstract 971]

  40. Rubin GS (2001) Vision rehabilitation for patients with age-related macular degeneration. Eye 15:430–435

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  41. Rubin GS, Turano K (1994) Low-vision reading with sequential word presentation. Vis Res 34:1723–1733

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  42. Rumney NJ (1995) Using visual thresholds to establish low vision performance. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt 15:S14–S18

    Google Scholar 

  43. Rumney NJ, Leat SJ (1994) Why do low vision patients still read slowly with a low vison aid? In: Kooijman A et al. (ed) Low vision research and new development in rehabilitation. IOP Press, Amsterdam, pp 269–274

    Google Scholar 

  44. Sloan LL, Brown DJ (1963) Reading cards for selection of optical aids for the partially sighted. Am J Ophthalmol 55:1187–1199

    Google Scholar 

  45. Sommerhalder J, Rappaz B, De Haller R, Fornos AP, Safran AB, Pelizzone M (2004) Simulation of artificial vision. II. Eccentric reading of full-page text and the learning of this task. Vis Res 44:1693–1706

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Stelmack J, Stelmack JR, Fraim M, Warrington J (1987) Clinical use of the Pepper Visual Skills for Reading Test in low vision rehabilitation. Am J Optom Physiol Opt 64:829–831

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  47. Stifter E, Sacu S, Weghaupt H, König F, Richter-Müksch S, Thaler A, Velikay-Parel M, Radner W (2004) Reading performance depending on the type of cataract and its predictability. J Cataract Refract Surg 30:1259–1267

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Stifter E, König F, Lang T, Bauer P, Richter-Müksch S, Thaler A, Velikay-Parel M, Radner W (2004) Reliability of a standardized reading chart system: variance component analysis, test-retest and inter-chart reliability. Graefes Arch Clin Exp Ophthalmol 242:31–39

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Sunness JS, Appelgate CA, Haselwood D, Rubin GS (1996) Fixation pattern and reading rate in eyes with central scotomas from advanced atrophic age-related macular degeneration and Stargardt disease. Ophthalmology 103:1458–1466

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  50. Timberlake GT, Mainster MA, Peli E, Auglière RA, Essock EA, Arend LE (1986) Reading with a macular scotoma. I. Retinal location of scotoma and fixation area. Investig Ophthalmol Vis Sci 27:1137–1147

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  51. Tripathy SP, Cavanagh P (2002) The extent of crowding in peripheral vision does not scale with target size. Vis Res 42:2357–2369

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Toet A, Levi DM (1992) The two-dimensional shape of spatial interaction zones in the parafovea. Vis Res 32:1349–1357

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  53. Townsend JT, Taylor SG, Brown DR (1971) Lateral masking for letters with unlimited viewing time. Percept Psychophys 10:375–378

    Google Scholar 

  54. Watson G, Baldasare J, Whittaker S (1990) The validity and clinical uses of the Pepper visual skills for reading test. J Vis Impair Blind 84:119–123

    Google Scholar 

  55. Whittaker SG, Lovie-Kitchin J (1993) Visual requirements for reading. Optom Vis Sci 70:54–65

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  56. Whittaker SG, Budd J, Cummings RW (1988) Eccentric fixation with macular scotoma. Investig Ophthalmol Vis Sci 29:268–278

    CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This investigation was supported by the Pro Visu Foundation, the Association pour le Bien des Aveugles, and the Fondation en Faveur des Aveugles, Geneva, Switzerland.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Anouk Déruaz.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Déruaz, A., Goldschmidt, M., Mermoud, C. et al. The relationship between word length and threshold character size in patients with central scotoma and eccentric fixation. Graefe's Arch Clin Exp Ophthalmo 244, 570–576 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00417-005-0111-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00417-005-0111-7

Keywords

Navigation