Abstract
Everyone experiences the natural ebb and flow of task-unrelated thoughts. Given how common the fluctuations in these thoughts are, surprisingly, we know very little about how they shape individuals’ responses to alcohol use. Here, we investigated if mind wandering is associated with a risk of developing problematic alcohol use. We undertook an online survey among the general population in China (N = 1123) and Germany (N = 1018) from December 2021 to February 2022 and examined the subjective experience of mind wandering and problematic alcohol use through the Mind Wandering Questionnaire (MWQ) and the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT). We compared mind wandering and problematic alcohol use between two countries and investigated the association between MWQ scores with AUDIT scores. We found higher scores on the MWQ and a high percentage of problematic alcohol use (i.e., AUDIT score ≥ 8) in Germany (22.5%) as compared to in China (14.5%). Higher self-reported mind wandering was associated with higher AUDIT scores. AUDIT scores were increased mostly in male, elder, and high-mind wandering people. Our findings highlight that mind wandering and problematic alcohol use enhanced in Germany as compared to in China. Our study sheds light on the relationship between mind wandering and problematic alcohol use that may help to further investigate causal effects of interventions.
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction
Our minds wander approximately half of the time during a day [19]. Mind wandering refers to the occurrence of intrusive thoughts unrelated to the current task [43, 45]. It has been associated with craving [2, 27], inattention and impulsivity [1, 22], as well as with low executive control [30, 44, 45] and fluctuations in motivation [4]. Impairments in these emotional and cognitive domains are common in alcohol use disorder (AUD) [6, 13].
Indeed, aspects of mind wandering (namely intrusive thoughts) have been related to alcohol consumption before. Specifically, elaborated intrusion theory suggests the conscious experience of craving as a cycle of mental elaboration of an initial intrusive thought [16, 28]. Intrusive thoughts about alcohol occur when a person associates their drinking with certain internal (e.g., feeling stressed) or external cues (walking past a bar) [29]. These cues trigger intrusive thoughts about alcohol that can then be elaborated upon in a cognitive processing system through the sensory modalities of vision, hearing, taste, and smell [29]. Increasing elaboration of such thoughts competes for the same limited cognitive resources, which correlates to a feeling of craving and to consumption of alcohol [17, 28, 29]. Other cognitive triggers, such as high alcohol expectancies were associated with increased appetitive response toward alcohol cues [7, 15] which might facilitate current and future alcohol consumption [23, 38, 42].
Mindfulness has been found to reduce mind wandering [37] and is known to moderate the relationship between impulsivity and the ability to disengage from alcohol-related thoughts [36]. Mindfulness might, thus, break what would otherwise create a positive feedback loop of automatic impulses to drink alcohol and alcohol-related thoughts [9]. The recent studies show that brief mindfulness-based training can help prevent and reduce alcohol cravings [48].
Theoretical reasoning moreover suggests additional paths of how mind wandering may relate to alcohol consumption. First, increased mind wandering was associated with impulsivity trait [1] and reduced ability to inhibit habitual response tendencies reduced ability to inhibit habitual response tendencies [12], both of which are known to facilitate problematic alcohol consumption [3, 6, 42]. Indeed, previous research showed that alcohol expectancies and habitual decision making predict relapse in alcohol dependence [42]. Second, acute alcohol ingestion in a lab lead to a greater incidence of mind wandering [10, 41], suggesting that alcohol consumption may elicit mind wandering. However, it is unclear whether real-life risky alcohol use is associated with chronically increased mind wandering rates, and whether such an association is valid across different cultural contexts.
Mind wandering has been studied across Eastern and Western cultures including in China [46] and in Germany [49], suggesting that it is a universal phenomenon [11, 26]. Yet, there are differences in mind-wandering rates across eastern and western cultures, suggesting that European participants do mind-wander more than Asian participants [47]. No recent study has been carried out on how mind wandering is associated with alcohol consumption across eastern and western cultures.
AUD is one of most common substance use disorders in both China and Germany with high prevalence [34] and low treatment rate [14, 50]. According to the World Health Organization (WHO), the 12-month prevalence of AUD in 2016 was 4.4% in China and 6.8% in Germany [34]. The treatment rate remains dramatically low, with only 2.4% in China [50] and 9% in Germany [14], which implies low treatment seeking rates, a lack of treatment availability, and an underuse of medical care [14, 50].
During the COVID-19 pandemic, on the one hand, a decline in alcohol availability and affordability and fewer chances to socialize [39] may lead to a reduction in alcohol consumption [18]. On the other hand, COVID-related stress may increase excessive alcohol misuse that can escalate into AUD [5, 20].
In this study, we aim to investigate mind wandering, the rates of problematic alcohol use in China and Germany, and whether high-mind wandering rates are associated with problematic alcohol use. To this end, we assessed the association between trait levels of mind wandering, assessed via the mind wandering questionnaire (MWQ; [31]), and risky alcohol consumption, assessed via the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT; [40]). As has been shown previously, European participants tend to focus more on internal psychological states than Asian participants [47]. Consequently, we expected that participants in Germany would mind-wander more than participants in China. Consistent with a previous report [34], we expected that Germany would have a higher rate of problematic alcohol use compared with China. Importantly, we expected that higher trait mind wandering is associated with a higher risk for problematic alcohol use in both countries.
Methods
Participants and procedure
We conducted a cross-sectional Sino-German study on the general population. We created an anonymous online survey on the Wenjuanxing platform (https://www.wjx.cn) in China and the Unipark platform (https://www.unipark.com/en/) in Germany. Participants over 18 years of age were eligible to take part in this study. There were no other inclusion criteria. The online survey was conducted from December 2021 to February 2022. We recruited them through social media, advertisements, newsletters, and Prolific (https://www.prolific.co). Prolific is an online platform that helps researchers recruit participants. The participation in China was fully voluntary and there was no compensation. To make sure each participant only completed our survey once, the Wenjuanxing platform enabled control of metadata. Participants in Germany were voluntary and were compensated with payment to their Prolific accounts only once, which ensured that one individual could only participate once in our given study.
Our online survey consisted of a sociodemographic assessment (i.e., sex and age), the MWQ questionnaires, and the AUDIT. The study was approved by the Ethics Committees at Charité – Universitätsmedizin Berlin (registration number: EA2/143/20), at Freie Universität Berlin (registration number: 030/2022), and at Shanghai Mental Health Center (registration number: 2021ky-15).
Mind wandering and alcohol use disorders identification test
We used a short 5-item MWQ to capture mind wandering [31], which has been previously used in China and Germany [21, 25]. Each item was rated on a 6-point Likert scale from 1 (almost never) to 6 (almost always) and the total score of five items ranged from 5 to 30. We used a classical instrument developed by the WHO, the AUDIT, to capture problematic alcohol use among general populations [40]. It has been widely used in China and Germany [8, 24]. The AUDIT consists of ten items with scores ranging from 0 to 4, thus allowing results from 0 to 40. According to the WHO guidelines (https://auditscreen.org/about/scoring-audit/), a score of 8 or more indicates problematic alcohol use (scores from 8 to 14 suggest hazardous or harmful alcohol consumption and a score of 15 or more indicates the likelihood of moderate to severe AUD).
Data analysis
Statistical analysis was performed using R Statistical Software (version 4.1.0; R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria, www.r-project.org). Differences were considered statistically significant at p < 0.05 and highly statistically significant at p < 0.001. To test effects of sex, age, country, and mind wandering on AUDIT scores, we used independent t tests for groups with pairwise comparisons in sex (males versus females), age (younger versus elder with median splits), country (China versus Germany), and mind wandering levels (lower vs. higher with median splits using the whole sample). Two-tailed hypothesis testing was used. To test if mind wandering scores had an influence on AUDIT scores, we ran a multiple linear regression model with significance testing implemented as a permutation test (lmPerm-package in R). In this model, we used “AUDIT score” as the outcome and included the main effects of “MWQ score”, “country” (China versus Germany; effect coding: -0.5 vs. + 0.5) as well as their interaction effect of “MWQ score × country” (to compare the relationship of mind wandering and problematic alcohol use between China and Germany), controlling for “sex” (males versus females; effect coding: -0.5 vs. + 0.5) and “age” (continuous). The intercept of effects coding represented the mean of two conditions, not just the mean of one condition. All continuous predictors were mean-centred for analysis. To test the assumption of having no multicollinearity, we calculated the variance inflation factor (VIF) values for all independent variables of the model.
Results
Group description
1123 participants in China (700 females; age range: 18–78, Mean = 28.80, SD = 11.54) and 1018 participants in Germany (514 females; age range: 18–80, Mean = 28.71, SD = 9.07) completed our survey from December 2021 to February 2022. Regarding comparison of social-demographic variables between China and Germany, we did not find a significant difference of “age” (t(2139) = 0.20, p = 0.84), but significant differences in “sex” (χ2 = 27.66, p < 0.001) between China and Germany, as shown in Table 1. Moreover, we found that participants in Germany reported higher levels of mind wandering as compared to participants in China, t(2139) = -14.06, p < 0.001. Participants in Germany reported higher AUDIT score as compared to participants in China, t(2139) = -9.43, p < 0.001. In Germany, 22.5% of participants reported problematic alcohol use (i.e., AUDIT score ≥ 8), ratings substantially higher than in China, where 14.5% of participants reported problematic alcohol use (χ2 = 22.21, p < 0.001). The proportion of participants with hazardous or harmful alcohol use (i.e., AUDIT score 8–14) in Germany (17.2%) was higher than in China (8.4%, χ2 = 37.01, p < 0.001). However, as for the proportions of participants with the likelihood of moderate to severe AUD (i.e., AUDIT score ≥ 15), there were no significant difference between two countries (6.1% in China vs. 5.3% in Germany, χ2 = 54.89, p = 0.459). In general, there were sex and age differences in AUDIT scores. Male participants reported relatively higher AUDIT score as compared to females, t(2130) = 12.32, p < 0.001. Elder participants reported relatively higher AUDIT score as compared to younger ones, t(2030) = -5.94, p < 0.001.
High-mind wandering respondents are at high risk to report problematic alcohol use
The risk for problematic alcohol use significantly differed between low versus high-mind wandering groups, t (1956) = -3.75, p < 0.001. As is shown in Fig. 1, individuals with higher mind wandering reported higher AUDIT scores (mean = 4.50, SD = 5.35) as compared to those with lower mind wandering (mean = 3.62, SD = 5.00). The high versus low-mind wandering groups differed in the proportions of participants reporting problematic alcohol use: In the high-mind wandering group, 20.8% of participants reported problematic alcohol use; in the low-mind wandering group, 15.8% of participants reported problematic alcohol use (χ2 = 8.04, p = 0.005).
The effect of mind wandering levels on the risk for problematic alcohol use. For the comparison between high (N = 893) versus low (N = 1065) mind wandering groups, each participant’s self-reported mind wandering score (MWQ) was ranked higher versus lower than the median (183 participants’ data at the median were removed). ***, p < 0.001
Increased mind wandering correlated with high AUDIT scores
The multiple linear model (F = 53.5, p < 0.001) was shown in Table 2. There were no significant associations among the predictor variables (VIF values < 1.16). In general, we found that the main effects of “mind wandering” (b = 0.12, p < 0.001), “country” (b = 1.42, p < 0.001) and their interaction effect of “mind wandering × country” (b = 0.11, p < 0.001) were significant predictors of high AUDIT scores after controlling for sociodemographic differences (i.e., sex and age, both p values < 0.001), shown in Fig. 2. An interaction effect of “mind wandering × country” indicated that participants in Germany with the higher mind wandering scores had higher AUDIT scores as compared to participants in China.
The effect of mind wandering on problematic alcohol use in China and in Germany. The Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT) score is displayed as a function of the mind wandering (MWQ) score, capturing problematic alcohol use among general populations, for China (N = 1123; red) versus Germany (N = 1018; blue)
Discussion
We conducted a cross-culture study evaluating mind wandering, the rates of problematic alcohol use, and the impact of mind wandering on problematic alcohol use. In line with our hypothesis, we found Germany to have a higher mind wandering level and a higher rate of problematic alcohol use as compared to China. High-mind wandering was associated with high AUDIT scores. Male, old, and high-mind wandering people were particularly at risk for problematic alcohol use.
Our findings on a high-mind wandering level in Germany as compared to China are consistent with previous cross-cultural studies on mind wandering [47]. Such differences may contribute to the nature of eastern and western thought process [32] which requires further elucidation.
Our findings on a higher rate of hazardous or harmful alcohol use in Germany (17.2%) as compared to in China (8.4%) reflect a generally higher alcohol consumption in Germany: In 2019, the annual alcohol per capita consumption in Germany (12.79 L of pure alcohol) was higher than in China (6.04 L) [33]. Moreover, in our data the rates of participants with likelihoods of moderate to severe AUD (based on the AUDIT) in China and in Germany were 6.1% and 5.3%, respectively. A previous WHO report showed that the 12-month prevalence of people with AUD in 2016 in China was 4.4% and in Germany was 6.8% [34]. Our findings are based on the AUDIT scores rather than a clinical diagnosis of AUD. AUDIT is a screening tool for unhealthy alcohol use, defined as risky or hazardous consumption or any AUD. Our results may lay the foundation for future studies on integrating a diagnostic tool.
Our findings provide new insights into the relevance of mind wandering in problematic alcohol use. We found that high-mind wandering was associated with high AUDIT scores in both countries. Respondents in Germany reported higher levels of mind wandering as compared to respondents in China. Moreover, we found high-mind wandering people were particularly at risk for problematic alcohol use. Our results suggest that individuals with high levels of mind wandering may be more prone to develop problematic alcohol use than those with low levels of mind wandering.
The causal direction of the found association between mind wandering and alcohol use remains open to further investigation: as one possibility, alcohol intake may increase mind wandering, possibly due to acute effects as alcohol intake may enhance mind wandering [10, 41], or due to chronic effects, since long-term alcohol consumption may lead to brain damage of executive control functions [35], which may enhance mind wandering rates. As a second possibility, mind wandering may increase alcohol consumption: during craving, people may wander their minds for alcoholic drinks/drinking situations. Moreover, increased mind wandering was associated with impulsivity traits [1] and reduced ability to inhibit habitual response tendencies [12], which may increase alcohol use [3, 6, 42]. A longitudinal study with repeated measurements may be helpful to disentangle correlation from causation. Future work may also explore the content of mind wandering, i.e., whether minds wander for alcohol-related content (reflecting craving), and whether acute mind wandering increases impulsivity and habitual choice to facilitate drinking.
There were two main limitations in this study. Firstly, we relied on convenience sampling and the generalizability and representativeness are limited in our sample (e.g., population with a mean age of 28.76 years; standard deviation = 10.44). Therefore, we cannot exclude the observed effects apply to a particular subpopulation. To avoid convenience sampling bias, we took multiple diverse samples (i.e., samples across two countries in China and in Germany) as a larger sample size. Secondly, the association between mind wandering and alcohol use is of correlational nature and does not support conclusions about causation as mentioned above. Our study did not inquire about other factors that have also been associated with alcohol use/problems (e.g., motivation, impulsivity, executive control). Therefore, we cannot exclude that the association of mind wandering with problematic alcohol use is mediated by other factors including attention, impulsivity traits or levels of executive control. Although with these limitations, we reported the rates of mind wandering and problematic alcohol use in China and Germany. In addition, the association between mind wandering and problematic alcohol use may inspire a new direction on alcohol use disorder and related interventions.
Data Availability
Derived data supporting the findings of this study are available from the first authors, S.L. and R.L., upon reasonable request.
References
Arabacı G, Parris BA (2018) Probe-caught spontaneous and deliberate mind wandering in relation to self-reported inattentive, hyperactive and impulsive traits in adults. Sci Rep 8:1–10
Barré T, Ramier C, Mounir I, Renaud D, Menvielle L, Marcellin F, Carrieri P, Protopopescu C, Cherikh F (2022) Examining the relationships between mindfulness and tobacco craving factors. Subst Use Misuse 57:656–659
Bernard L, Cyr L, Bonnet-Suard A, Cutarella C, Bréjard V (2021) Drawing alcohol craving process: a systematic review of its association with thought suppression, inhibition and impulsivity. Heliyon 7:e05868
Brosowsky NP, DeGutis J, Esterman M, Smilek D, Seli P (2020) Mind wandering, motivation, and task performance over time: Evidence that motivation insulates people from the negative effects of mind wandering. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice
Clay JM, Parker MO (2020) Alcohol use and misuse during the covid-19 pandemic: a potential public health crisis? Lancet Public Health 5:e259
Dick DM, Smith G, Olausson P, Mitchell SH, Leeman RF, O’Malley SS, Sher K (2010) Understanding the construct of impulsivity and its relationship to alcohol use disorders. Addict Biol 15:217–226
Drobes DJ, Carter AC, Goldman MS (2009) Alcohol expectancies and reactivity to alcohol-related and affective cues. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 17:1–9
Dybek I, Bischof G, Grothues J, Reinhardt S, Meyer C, Hapke U, John U, Broocks A, Hohagen F, Rumpf H-J (2006) The reliability and validity of the alcohol use disorders identification test (AUDIT) in a german general practice population sample. J Stud Alcohol 67:473–481
Field M, Cox WM (2008) Attentional bias in addictive behaviors: A review of its development, causes, and consequences. Drug Alcohol Depend 97:1–20
Finnigan F, Schulze D, Smallwood J (2007) Alcohol and the wandering mind: A new direction in the study of alcohol on attentional lapses. Int J Disabil Human Dev 6:189–200
Gonçalves ÓF, Rêgo G, Oliveira-Silva P, Leite J, Carvalho S, de Souza-Queiroz J, Fregni F, Amaro E Jr, Boggio PS (2017) Is the relationship between mind wandering and attention culture-specific? Psychol Neurosci 10:132–143
Hawkins GE, Mittner M, Forstmann BU, Heathcote A (2022) Self-reported mind wandering reflects executive control and selective attention. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review:1–14
Heinz A, Beck A, Halil MG, Pilhatsch M, Smolka MN, Liu S (2019) Addiction as learned behavior patterns. J Clin Med 8:1086
Hoffmann T, Voigt K, Kugler J, Peschel L, Bergmann A, Riemenschneider H (2019) Are german family practitioners and psychiatrists sufficiently trained to diagnose and treat patients with alcohol problems? BMC Fam Pract 20:1–7
Hogarth L, Field M, Rose AK (2013) Phasic transition from goal-directed to habitual control over drug-seeking produced by conflicting reinforcer expectancy. Addict Biol 18:88–97
Kavanagh DJ, Andrade J, May J (2005) Imaginary relish and exquisite torture: The elaborated intrusion theory of desire. Psychol Rev 112:446–467
Kavanagh DJ, May J, Andrade J (2009) Tests of the elaborated intrusion theory of craving and desire: features of alcohol craving during treatment for an alcohol disorder. Br J Clin Psychol 48:241–254
Kilian C, Rehm J, Allebeck P, Braddick F, Gual A, Barták M, Bloomfield K, Gil A, Neufeld M, O’Donnell A (2021) Alcohol consumption during the COVID-19 pandemic in Europe: A large-scale cross-sectional study in 21 countries. Addiction 116:3369–3380
Killingsworth MA, Gilbert DT (2010) A wandering mind is an unhappy mind. Science 330:932
Koopmann A, Georgiadou E, Reinhard I, Müller A, Lemenager T, Kiefer F, Hillemacher T (2021) The effects of the lockdown during the COVID-19 pandemic on alcohol and tobacco consumption behavior in Germany. Eur Addict Res 27:242–256
Kuschpel MS, Liu S, Schad DJ, Heinzel S, Heinz A, Rapp MA (2015) Differential effects of wakeful rest, music and video game playing on working memory performance in the n-back task. Front Psychol 6:1683
Lanier J, Noyes E, Biederman J (2021) Mind wandering (internal distractibility) in ADHD: A literature review. J Atten Disord 25:885–890
Leigh BC (1989) Attitudes and expectancies as predictors of drinking habits: A comparison of three scales. J Stud Alcohol 50:432–440
Li Q, Babor TF, Hao W, Chen X (2011) The Chinese translations of alcohol use disorders identification test (AUDIT) in China: A systematic review. Alcohol Alcohol 46:416–423
Luo Y, Zhu R, Ju E, You X (2016) Validation of the chinese version of the mind-wandering questionnaire (MWQ) and the mediating role of self-esteem in the relationship between mind-wandering and life satisfaction for adolescents. Personality Individ Differ 92:118–122
Martinon LM, Smallwood J, Hamilton C, Riby LM (2019) Frogs’ legs versus roast beef: How culture can influence mind-wandering episodes across the lifespan. Eur J Psychol 15:211
May J, Andrade J, Kavanagh DJ, Feeney GF, Gullo MJ, Statham DJ, Skorka-Brown J, Connolly JM, Cassimatis M, Young RM (2014) The craving experience questionnaire: A brief, theory-based measure of consummatory desire and craving. Addiction 109:728–735
May J, Andrade J, Panabokke N, Kavanagh D (2004) Images of desire: Cognitive models of craving. Memory 12:447–461
May J, Kavanagh DJ, Andrade J (2015) The elaborated intrusion theory of desire: A 10-year retrospective and implications for addiction treatments. Addict Behav 44:29–34
McVay JC, Kane MJ (2010) Does mind wandering reflect executive function or executive failure? Comment on smallwood and schooler (2006) and watkins (2008). Psychol Bull 136:188–197
Mrazek MD, Phillips DT, Franklin MS, Broadway JM, Schooler JW (2013) Young and restless: Validation of the mind-wandering questionnaire (mwq) reveals disruptive impact of mind-wandering for youth. Front Psychol 4:560
Nisbett E (2003) The geography of thought: How asians and westerns think differently. Free Press, New York, NY
World Health Organization (2021) Alcohol, total per capita (15+) consumption (in litres of pure alcohol).
World Health Organization (2019) Global status report on alcohol and health 2018.
Oscar-Berman M, Marinkovic K (2003) Alcoholism and the brain: An overview. Alcohol Res Health 27:125–133
Ostafin BD, Kassman KT, Wessel I (2013) Breaking the cycle of desire: Mindfulness and executive control weaken the relation between an implicit measure of alcohol valence and preoccupation with alcohol-related thoughts. Psychol Addict Behav 27:1153–1158
Rahl HA, Lindsay EK, Pacilio LE, Brown KW, Creswell JD (2017) Brief mindfulness meditation training reduces mind wandering: The critical role of acceptance. Emotion 17:224–230
Reese FL, Chassin L, Molina B (1994) Alcohol expectancies in early adolescents: Predicting drinking behavior from alcohol expectancies and parental alcoholism. J Stud Alcohol 55:276–284
Rehm J, Kilian C, Ferreira-Borges C, Jernigan D, Monteiro M, Parry CD, Sanchez ZM, Manthey J (2020) Alcohol use in times of the covid 19: Implications for monitoring and policy. Drug Alcohol Rev 39:301–304
Saunders JB, Aasland OG, Babor TF, De la Fuente JR, Grant M (1993) Development of the alcohol use disorders identification test (AUDIT): Who collaborative project on early detection of persons with harmful alcohol consumption-ii. Addiction 88:791–804
Sayette MA, Reichle ED, Schooler JW (2009) Lost in the sauce: The effects of alcohol on mind wandering. Psychol Sci 20:747–752
Sebold M, Nebe S, Garbusow M, Guggenmos M, Schad DJ, Beck A, Kuitunen-Paul S, Sommer C, Frank R, Neu P, Zimmermann US, Rapp MA, Smolka MN, Huys QJM, Schlagenhauf F, Heinz A (2017) When habits are dangerous: Alcohol expectancies and habitual decision making predict relapse in alcohol dependence. Biol Psychiat 82:847–856
Seli P, Kane MJ, Smallwood J, Schacter DL, Maillet D, Schooler JW, Smilek D (2018) Mind-wandering as a natural kind: A family-resemblances view. Trends Cogn Sci 22:479–490
Smallwood J (2010) Why the global availability of mind wandering necessitates resource competition: Reply to mcvay and kane. Psychol Bull 136:202–207
Smallwood J, Schooler JW (2006) The restless mind. Psychol Bull 132:946–958
Song X, Wang X (2012) Mind wandering in chinese daily lives–an experience sampling study. PLoS ONE 7:e44423
Sude DJ (2015) Culture influences rates of mind wandering. University of British Columbia Library, Vancouver
Tapper K (2018) Mindfulness and craving: Effects and mechanisms. Clin Psychol Rev 59:101–117
Tusche A, Smallwood J, Bernhardt BC, Singer T (2014) Classifying the wandering mind: Revealing the affective content of thoughts during task-free rest periods. Neuroimage 97:107–116
Xiang YT, Ma X, Lu JY, Cai ZJ, Li SR, Xiang YQ, Guo HL, Hou YZ, Li ZB, Li ZJ (2009) Alcohol-related disorders in Beijing, China: Prevalence, socio-demographic correlates, and unmet need for treatment. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 33:1111–1118
Acknowledgements
This research was supported by internal research funds from Charité – Universitätsmedizin Berlin (S.L.); Shanghai Municipal Science and Technology Major Project (2018SHZDZX05), Shanghai Clinical Research Center for Mental Health (M.Z., 19MC1911100), and Shanghai Shenkang Hospital Development Center (M.Z., SHDC2020CR3045B).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Ethical standards
The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008. All participants gave their informed consent prior to their inclusion in the study.
Rights and permissions
Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.
About this article
Cite this article
Liu, S., Li, R., Wegner, L. et al. High-mind wandering correlates with high risk for problematic alcohol use in China and Germany. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-023-01555-4
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-023-01555-4