Advertisement

Journal of Comparative Physiology A

, Volume 189, Issue 11, pp 801–809 | Cite as

Long-distance navigation in the wandering desert spider Leucorchestris arenicola: can the slope of the dune surface provide a compass cue?

  • T. Nørgaard
  • J. R. Henschel
  • R. WehnerEmail author
Original Paper

Abstract

Males of the nocturnal spider Leucorchestris arenicola (Araneae: Sparassidae) wander long distances over seemingly featureless dune surfaces in the Namib Desert searching for females. The spiders live in burrows to which they return after nearly every such excursion. While the outward path of an excursion may be a meandering search, the return path is often a nearly straight line leading towards the burrow. This navigational behaviour resembles that of path integration known from other arthropods, though on a much larger scale (over tens to hundreds of meters). Theoretically, precise navigation by path integration over long distances requires an external compass in order to adjust for inevitable accumulation of navigational errors. As a first step towards identifying any nocturnal compass cues used by the male spiders, a method for detailed 3-D recordings of the spider’s paths was developed. The 3-D reconstructions of the paths revealed details about the processes involved in the spiders’ nocturnal way of navigation. Analyses of the reconstructed paths suggest that gravity (slope of the dune surface) is an unlikely parameter used in path integration by the L. arenicola spiders.

Keywords

Arachnidae Gravity Homing Path integration Slope 

Notes

Acknowledgements

We thank the Gobabeb Training and Research Centre and the Ministry of Environment and Tourism for permission to work in the Namib-Naukluft Park, the Swiss National Science Foundation for funding the project (grant no. 31-61844.00 to R.W.), the Department of Zoology, University of Zürich, Switzerland and the Department of Zoology, University of Aarhus, Denmark, for equipment and infrastructural support. We are grateful also to Torben Geilman and Klaus Birkhofer for valuable help and fruitful discussions. This fieldwork complies with Namibian law.

References

  1. Bartels M (1929) Sinnesphysiologische und physiologische Untersuchungen an der Trichterspinne Agelena labyrinthica (Cl.). Z Vergl Physiol 10:527–591Google Scholar
  2. Barth FG (2002) A spider’s world, senses and behavior. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg NewYorkGoogle Scholar
  3. Benhamou S, Séguinot V (1995) How to find one’s way in the labyrinth of path integration models. J Theor Biol 174:463–466CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Benhamou S, Sauvé JP, Bovet P (1990) Spatial memory in large-scale movements: efficiency and limitations of the egocentric coding process. J Theor Biol 145:1–12Google Scholar
  5. Boles LC, Lohmann KJ (2003) True navigation and magnetic maps in spiny lobsters. Nature 421:60–63CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Brownell PH (1977) Compressional and surface waves in sand: used by desert scorpions to locate prey. Science 197:479–482Google Scholar
  7. Brownell PH (2001) Sensory ecology and orientational behaviors. In: Brownell PH, Polis GA (eds) Scorpion biology and research. Oxford University Press, pp 159–183Google Scholar
  8. Collett M, Collett TS, Wehner R (1999) Calibration of vector navigation in desert ants. Curr Biol 9:1031–1034Google Scholar
  9. Dacke M, Nilsson D-E, Warrant EJ, Blest AD, Land MF, O’Carroll DC (1999) Built-in polarizers form part of a compass organ in spiders. Nature 401:470–473CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Dyer FC (1996) Spatial memory and navigation by honeybees on the scale of the foraging range. J Exp Biol:199:147–154Google Scholar
  11. Frier HJ, Edwards E, Smith C, Neale S, Collett TS (1996) Magnetic compass cues and visual pattern learning in honeybees. J Exp Biol 199:1353–1361PubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Frisch K von (1967) The dance language and orientation of bees. The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MassachusettsGoogle Scholar
  13. Fukushi T (2001) Homing in wood ants, Formica japonica: use of the skyline panorama. J Exp Biol 204:2063–2072PubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Gal J, Horvath G, Barta A, Wehner R (2001) Polarization of the moonlit clear night sky measured by full-sky imaging polarimetry at full moon: comparison of the polarization of moonlit and sunlit skies. J Geophys Res D19 106:22647–22653Google Scholar
  15. Görner P, Class B (1985) Homing behavior and orientation in the funnel-web spider, Agelena labyrinthica Clerck. In: Barth FG (ed) Neurobiology of arachnids. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York, pp 275–297Google Scholar
  16. Henschel JR (1987) Sand-burrowing spiders: how do they do it? Bull Desert Res Unit 7:11Google Scholar
  17. Henschel JR (1990a) The biology of Leucorchestris arenicola (Araneae: Heteropodidae), a burrowing spider of the Namib Desert. In: Seely MK (ed) Namib ecology: 25 years of Namib research. Transvaal Museum monograph no. 7. Transvaal Museum, Pretoria, pp 115–127Google Scholar
  18. Henschel JR (1990b) Spiders wheel to escape. S Afr J Sci 86:151–152Google Scholar
  19. Henschel JR (1994) Diet and foraging behaviour of huntsman spiders in the Namib dunes (Araneae: Heteropodidae). J Zool (Lond) 234:239–251Google Scholar
  20. Henschel JR (2002) Long distance wandering and mating by the dancing white lady spider (Leucorchestris arenicola) (Araneae, Sparrassidae) across Namib dunes. J Arachnol 30:321–330Google Scholar
  21. Hill DE (1979) Orientation by jumping spiders of the genus Phidippus (Araneae: Salticidae). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 5:301–322Google Scholar
  22. Jäger P (1999) Sparassidae - the valid scientific name for the huntsman spiders (Arachnida: Araneae). Arachnol Mitt 17:1–10Google Scholar
  23. Lawrence RF (1962) Spiders of the Namib Desert. Ann Transvaal Mus 10:197–211Google Scholar
  24. Lawrence RF (1965) New and little known Arachnida from the Namib Desert, S.W. Africa. Sci Pap Namib Desert Res Stn 27:1–12Google Scholar
  25. Menzel R, Geiger K, Joerges J, Müller U, Chittka L (1998) Bees travel novel homeward routes by integrating separately acquired vector memories. Anim Behav 55:139–152Google Scholar
  26. Mittelstaedt H (1985) Analytical cybernetics of spider navigation. In: Barth FG (ed) Neurobiology of arachnids. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York, pp 298–316Google Scholar
  27. Mittelstaedt H, Mittelstaedt ML (1982) Homing by path Integration. In: Papi F, Wallraff HG (eds) Avian navigation. International Symposium on Avian Navigation (ISAN), Tirrenia (Pisa). Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York, pp 290–297Google Scholar
  28. Mouritsen H, Frost BJ (2002) Virtual migration in tethered flying monarch butterflies reveals their orientation mechanisms. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 99:10162–10166CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Müller M, Wehner R (1988) Path integration in desert ants, Cataglyphis fortis. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 85:5278–5290Google Scholar
  30. Müller M, Wehner R (1994) The hidden spiral: systematic search and path integration in desert ants, Cataglyphis fortis. J Comp Physiol A 175:525–530Google Scholar
  31. Papke MD, Reichert SE, Schultz S (2001) An airborne female pheromone associated with male attraction and courtship in a desert spider. Anim Behav 61:877–886CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Pollard SD, Macnab AM, Jackson RR (1987) Communication with chemicals: pheromones and spiders. In: Nentwig W (ed) Ecophysiology of spiders. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York, pp 133–141Google Scholar
  33. Rickli M, Leuthold RH (1988) Homing in harvester termites: evidence of magnetic orientation. Ethology 77:209–216Google Scholar
  34. Robinson MD, Seely MK (1980) Physical and biotic environments of the southern Namib dune ecosystem. J Arid Environ 3:183–203Google Scholar
  35. Rossel S, Wehner R (1984) Celestial orientation in bees: the use of spectral cues. J Comp Physiol A 155:605–613Google Scholar
  36. Rossel S, Wehner R (1986) Polarization vision in bees. Nature 323:128–131Google Scholar
  37. Seyfarth EA, Barth FG (1972) Compound slit sense organs on the spider leg: mechanoreceptors involved in kinesthetic orientation. J Comp Physiol 78:176–191Google Scholar
  38. Seyfarth EA, Hergenröder R, Ebbes H, Barth FG (1982) Ideothetic orientation of a wandering spider: compensation of detours and estimates of goal distance. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 11:139–148Google Scholar
  39. Srinivasan MV, Zhang SW, Lehrer M, Collett TS (1996) Honeybee navigation en route to the goal: visual flight control and odometry. J Exp Biol 199:237–244PubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Ugolini A, Pezzani A (1995) Magnetic compass and learning of the Y-axis (sea-land) direction in the marine isopod Idotea baltica basteri. Anim Behav 50:295–300CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Vollrath F, Nørgaard T, Krieger M (2002) Radius orientation in the garden cross spider Araneus diadematus. In: Toft S, Scharff N (eds) Proceedings of the 19th European Colloquium of Arachnology, Aarhus 17–22 July 2000. Aarhus University Press, pp 107–116Google Scholar
  42. Wehner R (1992) Arthropods. In: Papi F (ed) Animal Homing. Chapman and Hall, London, pp 45–144Google Scholar
  43. Wehner R (1994) The polarization-vision project: championing organismic biology. Fortschr Zool 39:103–143Google Scholar
  44. Wehner R (1997) The ant’s celestial compass system: spectral and polarization channels. In: Lehrer M (ed) Orientation and communication in arthropods. Birkhäuser, Basel, pp 145–185Google Scholar
  45. Wehner R, Duelli P (1971) The spatial orientation of desert ants, Cataglyphis bicolor, before sunrise and after sunset. Experientia 27:1364–1366Google Scholar
  46. Wehner R, Wehner S (1986) Path integration in desert ants. Approaching a long-standing puzzle in insect navigation. Monit Zool Ital 20:309–331Google Scholar
  47. Wehner R, Wehner S (1990) Insect navigation: use of maps or Ariadne’s thread? Ethol Ecol Evol 2:27–48Google Scholar
  48. Wehner R, Harkness RD, Schmid-Hempel P (1983) Foraging strategies in individually searching ants, Cataglyphis bicolor (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). Akad Wiss Lit Mainz Abh Math Naturwiss Kl. Fischer, StuttgartGoogle Scholar
  49. Wehner R, Michel B, Antonsen P (1996) Visual navigation in insects: coupling of egocentric and geocentric information. J Exp Biol 199:129–140PubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Wehner R, Gallizzi K, Frei C, Vesely M (2002) Calibration processes in desert ant navigation: vector courses and systematic search. J Comp Physiol A 188:683–693Google Scholar
  51. Wohlgemuth S, Ronacher B, Wehner R (2001) Ant odometry in the third dimension. Nature 411:795–798PubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Wohlgemuth S, Ronacher B, Wehner R (2002) Distance estimation in the third dimension in desert ants. J Comp Physiol A 188:273–281CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Zar JH (1999) Biostatistical analysis. Prentice-Hall, LondonGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2003

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of ZoologyUniversity of ZuerichZuerichSwitzerland
  2. 2.Gobabeb Training and Research CentreWalvis BayAfrica

Personalised recommendations