Advertisement

Journal of Mathematical Biology

, Volume 68, Issue 1–2, pp 505–531 | Cite as

On the maintenance of bird song dialects

  • Robert Planqué
  • Nicholas F. Britton
  • Hans Slabbekoorn
Article

Abstract

Many bird species, especially song birds but also for instance some hummingbirds and parrots, have noted dialects. By this we mean that locally a particular song is sung by the majority of the birds, but that neighbouring patches may feature different song types. Behavioural ecologists have been interested in how such dialects come about and how they are maintained for over 45 years. As a result, a great deal is known about different mechanisms at play, such as dispersal, assortative mating and learning of songs, and there are several competing hypotheses to explain the dialect patterns known in nature. There is, however, surprisingly little theoretical work testing these different hypotheses at present. We analyse the simplest kind of model that takes into account the most important biological mechanisms, and in which one may speak of dialects: a model in which there are but two patches, and two song types. It teaches us that a combination of little dispersal and strong assortative mating ensures dialects are maintained. Assuming a simple, linear frequency-dependent learning rule has little effect on the maintenance of dialects. A nonlinear learning rule, however, has dramatic consequences and greatly facilitates dialect maintenance. Adding fitness benefits for singing particular songs in a given patch also has a great impact. Now rare song types may invade and remain in the population.

Keywords

Mathematical biology Dialects Bird song Modeling  Difference equations Behavioural ecology 

Mathematics Subject Classification

37N25 92A18 

References

  1. Appleby BM, Redpath SM (1997) Indicators of male quality in the hoots of tawny owls (Strix aluco). J Raptor Res 31:65–70Google Scholar
  2. Baker MC (2003) Local similarity and geographic differences in a contact call of the galah (Cacatua roseicapilla assimilis) in Western Australia. Emu 103:233–237CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Baker MC, Cunningham MA (1985) The biology of bird-song dialects. Behav Brain Sci 8:85–133CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Baker MC, Spitler-Nabors KJ, Thompson ADJ, Cunningham MA (1987) Reproductive behaviour of female white-crowned sparrows: effect of dialects and synthetic hybrid songs. Anim Behav 35:1766–1774CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Baptista LF, Boarman WI, Kandianidis P (1983) Behavior and taxonomic status of Grayson’s dove. Auk 100:907–919Google Scholar
  6. Baptista LF, King JR (1980) Geographical variation in song and song dialects of the montane white-crowned sparrow. Condor 82:267–284CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Baptista LF, Petrinovich L (1984) Social interaction, sensitive phases and the song template hypothesis in the white-crowned sparrow. Anim Behav 32:172–181Google Scholar
  8. Bell DA, Trail PW, Baptista LF (1998) Song learning and vocal tradition in Nuttall’s white-crowned sparrows. Anim Behav 55:939–956Google Scholar
  9. Borror DJ (1956) Variation in Carolina Wren songs. Auk 73:211–229CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Boyd R, Richardson PJ (1985) Culture and the evolutionary process. Chicago University Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  11. Camazine S, Deneubourg J-L, Franks NR, Sneyd J, Theraulaz G, Bonabeau E (2001) Self-organization in biological systems. Princeton University Press, PrincetonGoogle Scholar
  12. Catchpole C, Slater B (1995) Bird song: biological themes and variations. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  13. Cunningham MA, Baker MC, Boardman TJ (1989) Microgeographic song variation in the Nuttall’s white-crowned sparrow. Condor 89:261–275Google Scholar
  14. Derryberry EP (2011) Male response to historical and geographical variation in bird song. Biol Lett 7:57–59CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Dingle C, Halfwerk W, Slabbekoorn H (2008) Habitat-dependent song divergence at subspecies level in grey-breasted wood-wren. J Evol Biol 21:1079–1089Google Scholar
  16. Dingle C, Poelstra JW, Halfwerk W, Brinkhuizen DM, Slabbekoorn H (2010) Asymmetric response patterns to subspeciesspecific song differences in allopatry and parapatry in the gray-breasted wood-wren. Evolution 64:3537–3548Google Scholar
  17. Edwards SV, Kingan SB, Calkins JD, Balakrishnan CN, Jennings WB, Swanson WJ, Sorenson MD (2005) Speciation in birds: genes, geography, and sexual selection. Proc Nat Acad Sci 102:6550–6557CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Ellers J, Slabbekoorn H (2003) Song divergence and male dispersal among bird populations: a spatially explicit model testing the role of vocal learning. Anim Behav 65:671–681CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Gaunt SLL, Baptista LF, Sánchez JE, Hernández D (1994) Song learning as evidenced from song sharing in two hummingbird species (Colibri coruscans and C. thalassinus). Auk 111:87–103CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Goldstein RB (1978) Geographic variation in Hoy call of Bobwhite Colinus virginianus. Auk 95:85–94CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Grant BR, Grant PR (1996) Cultural inheritance of song and its role in the evolution of Darwinś finches. Evolution 50:2471–2487CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Handford P (1988) Trill rate dialects in the rufous-collared sparrow, Zonotrichia capensis, in north-western Argentina. Can J Zool 66:2658–2670CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Harbison H, Nelson DA, Hahn TP (1999) Longterm persistence of song dialects in the mountain whitecrowned sparrow. Condor 101:133–148CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Kroodmsa DE (1985) Goegraphic variation in songs of the Bewicks Wren complex: a search for correlations with avifaunal complexity. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 16:143–150CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Kroodsma DE (1974) Song learning, dialects and dispersal in the Bewicks wren. Z. für Tierpsychologie 35:352–380Google Scholar
  26. Kroodsma DE (2004) The diversity and plasticity of birdsong. In: Marler P, Slabbekoorn H (eds) Nature’s Music. Elsevier Academic Press, Amsterdam, pp 108–131CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Kroodsma DE, Liu WC, Goodwin E, Bedell PA (1999) The ecology of song improvisation as illustrated by North American Sedge Wrens. Auk 116:373–386CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Lachlan R, Slater PJB (1999) The maintenance of vocal learning by gene-culture interaction: the cultural trap hypothesis. Proc Roy Soc London B 266:701–706CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Lachlan RF, Janik VM, Slater PJB (2004) The evolution of conformity-enforcing behaviour in cultural communication systems. Anim Behav 68:561–570CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Lachlan RF, Servedio MR (2004) Song learning accelerates allopatric speciation. Evolution 58(9): 2049–2063Google Scholar
  31. Lachlan RF, Slater PJB (2003) Song learning by chaffinches: how accurate, and from where? Anim Behav 65:957–969CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Luther D, Baptista LF (2010) Urban noise and the cultural evolution of bird songs. Proc Roy Soc London B 277:469–473CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. MacDougall-Shackleton EA, Derryberry EP, Hahn TP (2002) Nonlocal male mountain white-crowned sparrows have lower paternity and higher parasite loads than males singing local dialect. Behav Ecol 13:682–689CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Marler P (1970) A comparative approach to vocal learning: song development in white-crowned sparrows. J Comp Physiol Psychol 71:1–25CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Marler P, Peters S (1982) Developmental overproduction and selective attrition: new processes in the epigenesis of birdsong. Dev Psychiobiol 15:369–378CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Marler P, Slabbekoorn H (2004) Nature’s music. Elsevier Academic Press, AmsterdamGoogle Scholar
  37. Marler P, Tamura M (1962) Song “dialects” in three populations of white-crowned sparrows. Condor 64:368–377CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. McGregor PK, Krebs JR (1989) Song learning in adult great tits (Parus major): effect of neighbours. Behaviour 108:139–159CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Mountjoy DE, Lemon RE (1995) Extended song learning in wild European starlings. Anim Behav 49: 357–366Google Scholar
  40. Mundinger PC (1982) Microgeographic and macrogeographic variation in the acquired vocalizations of birds. In: Kroodsma DE, Miller EH, Quellet H (eds) Acoustic communication in birds, vol I. Academic Press, New York, pp 147–208Google Scholar
  41. Murray JD (2003) Mathematical biology II: spatial models and biomedical applications. Springer, BerlinGoogle Scholar
  42. Nelson DA (1992) Song overproduction and selective attrition lead to song sharing in the field sparrow (Spizella pusilla). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 30:415–424CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Nelson DA (2000) Song overproduction, selective attrition, and song dialects in the white-crowned sparrow. Anim Behav 60:887–898CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Nelson DA, Hallberg KI, Soha JA (2004) Cultural evolution of Puget Sound white-crowned sparrow song dialects. Ethology 110:879–908Google Scholar
  45. Nelson DA, Khanna H, Marler P (2001) Learning by instruction or selection: implications for patterns of geographic variation in bird song. Behaviour 138:1137–1160CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Nelson DA, Marler P, Morton ML (1996) Overproduction in song development: an evolutionary correlate with migration. Anim Behav 51:1127–1140CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Nottebohm F, Nottebohm ME (1978) Relationship between song repertoire and age in the canary, Serinus canarius. Z. für Tierpsychologie 46:298–305CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Payne RB (1978) Microgeographic variation in songs of the splendid sunbird Nectarinia coccinigaster: population phenetics, habitats, and song dialects. Behaviour 65:282–308CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Payne RB (1981) Song learning and social interaction in indigo buntings. Anim Behav 29:688–697CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Peake TM, McGregror PK (1999) Geographical variation in the vocalization of the corn crake Crex crex. Ethol Ecol Evol 11:123–137CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Podos J, Warren PS (2007) The evolution of geographic variation in birdsong. Adv Study Behav 37:403–458Google Scholar
  52. Price T (2008) Speciation in birds. Roberts and Company, Greenwood VillageGoogle Scholar
  53. Rivera-Gutierrez HF, Matthysen E, Adriaensen F, Slabbekoorn H (2010) Repertoire sharing and song similarity between great tit males decline with distance between forest fragments. Ethology 116: 951–960Google Scholar
  54. Slabbekoorn H, Jesse A, Bell D (2003) Microgeographic song variation in island populations of the white-crowned sparrow (Zonotrichia leucophrys nutalli): innovation through recombination. Behaviour 140:947–963CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Slabbekoorn H, Peet M (2003) Birds sing at higher pitch in urban noise. Nature 424:267CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Slabbekoorn H, Smith TB (2002) Bird song, ecology and speciation. Proc Roy Soc London B 357:493–503Google Scholar
  57. Strigul N (2009) Can imitation explain dialect origins? Ecol Model 220:2624–2639CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Williams JM, Slater PJB (1990) Modelling bird song dialects: the influence of repertoire size and number of neighbours. J Theor Biol 145:487–496CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Wright TF (1996) Regional dialects in the contact call of a parrot. Proc Roy Soc London B 263:867–872CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Yang X-J, Lei F-M, Wang G, Jesse AJ (2007) Syllable sharing and inter-individual syllable variation in Anna’s hummingbird Calypte anna songs, in San Francisco. California Folia Zool 56:307–318Google Scholar
  61. Zann R (1997) Vocal learning in wild and domesticated zebra finches: signature cues for kin recognition or epiphenomena? In: Snowdon CT, Hausberger M (eds) Social influences on vocal development. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 85–97CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  • Robert Planqué
    • 1
  • Nicholas F. Britton
    • 2
  • Hans Slabbekoorn
    • 3
  1. 1.Department of MathematicsVU University AmsterdamAmsterdamThe Netherlands
  2. 2.Department of Mathematical Sciences and Centre for Mathematical BiologyUniversity of BathBathUK
  3. 3.Institute of Biology LeidenLeiden UniversityLeidenThe Netherlands

Personalised recommendations