Skip to main content
Log in

Geographic variation in marmots’ alarm calls causes different responses

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Geographic variation in acoustic signals has been investigated for five decades to better understand the evolution of communication. When receivers are able to discriminate among signals and to react accordingly, geographic variation can have major impacts on the ability of conspecifics to communicate. Surprisingly, geographic variation in alarm calls and its consequences for the communication process have been so far neglected despite their crucial role on individual survival. Working with four wild populations of Alpine marmots (Marmota marmota), we found differences in the acoustic structure of their alarm calls. These differences cannot be explained by geographic or genetic distances but more likely by other mechanisms including random processes. Moreover, playback experiments provided evidence that receivers discriminate between alarm calls from their own versus other populations, with responses at lower intensity when the alarm calls played back originated from their own population. Research on the mechanistic causes of geographic variation and on the relationship between alarm call variation, familiarity, and intelligibility of signal and behavioral responses is now required to better understand how predation pressure, and more widely natural selection, could drive the evolution of communication.

Significance statement

Dialects (i.e., geographic variation) can have major impacts on the ability of conspecifics to communicate. Surprisingly, dialects in alarm calls have been neglected despite their crucial role on survival of individuals. Alpine marmots have dialects in alarm calls and discriminate their own dialects from others, being more frightened by alarm calls from another population than by those from their own. Confronted with an unknown dialect, marmots may adopt a self-preserving strategy and choose to run away before assessing the danger.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Baker MC (1982) Genetic population structure and vocal dialects in Zonotrichia (Emberizidae). In: Kroodsma DE, Miller EH (eds) Acoustic communication in birds, vol 2. Academic Press, New York, pp 209–235

    Google Scholar 

  • Baker MC, Cunningham MA (1985) The biology of bird-song dialects. Behav Brain Sci 8:85–133

    Google Scholar 

  • Baker AJ, Jenkins PF (1987) Founder effect and cultural evolution of songs in an isolated population of chaffinches, Fringilla coelebs, in the Chatham Islands. Anim Behav 35:1793–1803

    Google Scholar 

  • Becker PH (1982) The coding of species-specific characteristics in bird sounds. In: Kroodsma DE, Miller EH (eds) Acoustic communication in birds, vol 2. Academic press, New York, pp 213–252

    Google Scholar 

  • Bichet C, Sauzet S, Averty L, Dupont P, Ferrandiz-Rovira M, Ferrari C, Figueroa I, Tafani M, Rézouki C, López BC, Cohas A (2016) Multiple geographic origins and high genetic differentiation of the Alpine marmots reintroduced in the Pyrenees. Conserv Genet 17:1157–1169

    Google Scholar 

  • Blumstein DT (2007) The evolution, function, and meaning of marmot alarm communication. Adv Study Behav 37:371–400

    Google Scholar 

  • Blumstein DT, Armitage KB (1997) Alarm calling in yellow-bellied marmots: I. The meaning of situationally variable alarm calls. Anim Behav 53:143–171

    Google Scholar 

  • Blumstein DT, Munos O (2005) Individual, age and sex-specific information is contained in yellow-bellied marmot alarm calls. Anim Behav 69:353–361

    Google Scholar 

  • Blumstein DT, Nguyen KT, Martin JGA (2013) Ontogenetic variation of heritability and maternal effects in yellow-bellied marmot alarm calls. Proc R Soc B 280:20130176

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boul KE, Funk WC, Darst CR et al (2007) Sexual selection drives speciation in an Amazonian frog. Proc R Soc Lond B 274:399–406

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradley DW, Molles LE, Waas JR (2013) Local–foreign dialect discrimination and responses to mixed-dialect duets in the North Island Kōkako. Behav Ecol 24:570–578

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell GS (2004) Quantitative comparison of bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops spp.) whistles from three geographic regions. MS thesis, San Diego State University

  • Campbell P, Pasch B, Pino JL et al (2010) Geographic variation in the songs of neotropical singing mice: testing the relative importance of drift and local adaptation. Evolution 64:1955–1972

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Catchpole C, Slater P (2008) Bird song. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Cleator HJ, Stirling I, Smith TG (1989) Underwater vocalizations of the bearded seal (Erignathus barbatus). Can J Zool 67:1900–1910

    Google Scholar 

  • Colbeck GJ, Sillett TS, Webster MS (2010) Asymmetric discrimination of geographical variation in song in a migratory passerine. Anim Behav 80:311–318

    Google Scholar 

  • Conner DA (1982) Dialects versus geographic variation in mammalian vocalizations. Anim Behav 30:297–298

    Google Scholar 

  • Coss RG, McCowan B, Ramakrishnan U (2007) Threat-related acoustical differences in alarm calls by wild bonnet macaques (Macaca radiata) elicited by python and leopard models. Ethology 113:352–367

    Google Scholar 

  • Couturier M (1955) Acclimatation et acclimatement de la Marmotte des Alpes, Marmota marmota (Linné 1758), dans les Pyrénées françaises. Saugetierkd Mitt 3:105–108

    Google Scholar 

  • Daniel JC, Blumstein DT (1998) A test of the acoustic adaptation hypothesis in four species of marmots. Anim Behav 56:1517–1528

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Danner JE, Danner RM, Bonier F, Martin PR, Small TW, Moore IT (2011) Female, but not male, tropical sparrows respond more strongly to the local song dialect: implications for population divergence. Am Nat 178:53–63

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Davidson SM, Wilkinson GS (2002) Geographic and individual variation in vocalizations by male Saccopteryx bilineata (Chiroptera: Emballonuridae). J Mammal 83:526–535

    Google Scholar 

  • Dray S, Dufour AB (2007) The ade4 package: implementing the duality diagram for ecologists. J Stat Softw 22:1–20

    Google Scholar 

  • Dutour M, Lena JP, Lengagne T (2016) Mobbing behaviour varies according to predator dangerousness and occurrence. Anim Behav 119:119–124

    Google Scholar 

  • Dutour M, Lena JP, Lengagne T (2017) Mobbing behaviour in a passerine community increases with prevalence in predator diet. Ibis 159:324–330

    Google Scholar 

  • Eiler KC, Banack SA (2004) Variability in the alarm call of golden-mantled ground squirrels (Spermophilus lateralis and S. saturatus). J Mammal 85:43–50

    Google Scholar 

  • Francescoli G (2002) Geographic variation in vocal signals of Ctenomys pearsoni. Acta Theriol 47:35–44

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray DA (2005) Does courtship behavior contribute to species-level reproductive isolation in field crickets? Behav Ecol 16:201–206

    Google Scholar 

  • Halfwerk W, Slabbekoorn H (2009) A behavioural mechanism explaining noise-dependent frequency use in urban birdsong. Anim Behav 78:1301–1307

    Google Scholar 

  • Handford P (1988) Trill rate dialects in the Rufous-collared sparrow, Zonotrichia capensis, in northwestern Argentina. Can J Zool 66:2658–2670

    Google Scholar 

  • Hare JF, Warkentin KJ (2012) The song remains the same: juvenile Richardson’s ground squirrels do not respond differentially to mother’s or colony member’s alarm calls. Curr Zool 58:773–780

    Google Scholar 

  • Hothorn T, Bretz F, Westfall P (2008) Simultaneous inference in general parametric models. Biom J 50:346–363

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hunter ML, Krebs JR (1979) Geographical variation in the song of the great tit (Parus major) in relation to ecological factors. J Anim Ecol 48:759–785

    Google Scholar 

  • Hyman J (2005) Seasonal variation in response to neighbors and strangers by a territorial songbird. Ethology 111:951–961

    Google Scholar 

  • Irwin DE, Thimgan MP, Irwin JH (2008) Call divergence is correlated with geographic and genetic distance in greenish warblers (Phylloscopus trochiloides): a strong role for stochasticity in signal evolution? J Evol Biol 21:435–448

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Janik V, Slater P (2003) Traditions in mammalian and avian vocal communication. In: Fragaszy MD, Perry MD (eds) The biology of tradition: models and evidences. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 213–235

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirchhof J, Hammerschmidt K (2006) Functionally referential alarm calls in tamarins (Saguinus fuscicollis and Saguinus mystax) evidence from playback experiments. Ethology 112:346–354

    Google Scholar 

  • Krebs JR, Kroodsma DE (1980) Repertoires and geographical variation in bird song. Adv Study Behav 11:143–177

    Google Scholar 

  • Kroodsma DE (2004) The diversity and plasticity of birdsong. In: Marler P, Slabbekoorn H (eds) Nature’s music. Elsevier Academic, London, pp 108–130

    Google Scholar 

  • Lameira AR, Delgado RA, Wich SA (2010) Review of geographic variation in terrestrial mammalian acoustic signals: human speech variation in a comparative perspective. J Evol Psychol 8:309–332

    Google Scholar 

  • Lea AJ, Blumstein DT (2011) Age and sex influence marmot antipredator behavior during periods of heightened risk. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 65:1525–1533

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Leader N, Wright J, Yom-Tov Y (2002) Dialect discrimination by male Orange-tufted sunbirds (Nectarinia osea): reactions to own vs. neighbor dialects. Ethology 108:367–376

    Google Scholar 

  • Leader N, Geffen E, Mokady O, Yom-Tov Y (2008) Song dialects do not restrict gene flow in an urban population of the orange-tufted sunbird, Nectarinia osea. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 62:1299–1305

    Google Scholar 

  • Lengagne T (2001) Temporal stability over several years in the calls of eagle owl (Bubo bubo). Behaviour 138:1407–1419

    Google Scholar 

  • Lengagne T, Slater PJB (2002) The effects of rain on acoustic communication: tawny owls have good reason for calling less in wet weather. Proc R Soc Lond B 269:2121–2125

    Google Scholar 

  • Lengagne T, Aubin T, Jouventin P, Lauga J (1999) Acoustic communication in a king penguin colony: importance of bird location within the colony and of the body position of the listener. Polar Biol 21:262–268

    Google Scholar 

  • Lima SL, Dill LM (1990) Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation: a review and prospectus. Can J Zool 68:619–640

    Google Scholar 

  • Lin A, Liu H, Chang Y, Lu G, Feng J (2016) Behavioural response of the greater horseshoe bat to geographical variation in echolocation calls. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 70:1765–1776

    Google Scholar 

  • Lougheed SC, Handford P (1992) Vocal dialects and the structure of geographic variation in morphological and allozymic characters in the rufous-collared sparrow, Zonotrichia capensis. Evolution 46:1443–1456

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loughry WJ, Oeser M, Anderson CD, Hoogland JL (2019) The importance of individual variation in the alarm calls of Gunnison’s prairie dogs. Anim Behav 150:59–68

    Google Scholar 

  • Maeda T, Masataka N (2010) Locale-specific vocal behaviour of the tamarin (Saguinus I. labiatus). Ethology 75:25–30

    Google Scholar 

  • Manser MB (2001) The acoustic structure of suricates’ alarm calls varies with predator type and the level of response urgency. Proc R Soc Lond B 268:2315–2324

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mantel N (1967) The detection of disease clustering and a generalized regression approach. Cancer Res 27:209–220

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Matrosova VA, Volodin IA, Volodina EV, Babitsky AF (2007) Pups crying bass: vocal adaptation for avoidance of age-dependent predation risk in ground squirrels? Behav Ecol Sociobiol 62:181–191

    Google Scholar 

  • Matrosova VA, Pivanova SV, Savinetskaya LE et al (2012) The between-population variation of the alarm call in the speckled ground squirrel (Spermophilus suslicus, Rodentia, Sciuridae): effects of sex, age and body mass. Zool Zh 91:453–463

    Google Scholar 

  • Matrosova VA, Rusin MY, Volodina EV, Proyavka SV, Savinetskaya LE, Shekarova ON, Rashevska HV, Volodin IA (2016) Genetic and alarm call diversity across scattered populations of speckled ground squirrels (Spermophilus suslicus). Mamm Biol 81:255–265

    Google Scholar 

  • McCowan B, Hooper SL (2002) Individual acoustic variation in Belding’s ground squirrel alarm chirps in the High Sierra Nevada. J Acoust Soc Am 111:1157–1160

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mortega KG, Flinks H, Helm B (2014) Behavioural response of a migratory songbird to geographic variation in song and morphology. Front Zool 11:85

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Morton ES (1975) Ecological sources of selection on avian sounds. Am Nat 109:17–34

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundinger P (1982) Microgeographic and macrogeographic variation in the acquired vocalizations of birds. In: Kroodsma DE, Miller EH (eds) Acoustic communication in birds, vol 2. Academic Press, New York, pp 147–208

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicholls JA (2008) Site specificity in advertisement calls and responses to playbacks of local and foreign call variants in satin bowerbirds, Ptilonorhynchus violaceus. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 62:831–841

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicholls JA, Goldizen AW (2006) Habitat type and density influence vocal signal design in satin bowerbirds. J Anim Ecol 75:549–558

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nottebohm F (1969) The song of the chingolo, Zonotrichia capensis, in Argentina: description and evaluation of a system of dialects. Condor 71:299–315

    Google Scholar 

  • Oksanen J, Blanchet FG, Friendly M et al (2018) Vegan R package version 2.5-2. Community ecology package, https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=vegan

  • Parmentier E, Lagardère J, Vandewalle P et al (2005) Geographical variation in sound production in the anemonefish Amphiprion akallopisos. Proc R Soc Lond B 272:1697–1703

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Perla BS, Slobodchikoff CN (2002) Habitat structure and alarm call dialects in Gunnison’s prairie dog (Cynomys gunnisoni). Behav Ecol 13:844–850

    Google Scholar 

  • Perrin C, Allainé D, Leberre M (1993) Socio-spatial organization and activity distribution of the alpine marmot Marmota marmota - preliminary results. Ethology 93:21–30

    Google Scholar 

  • Perry EA, Terhune JM (1999) Variation of harp seal (Pagophilus groenlandicus) underwater vocalizations among three breeding locations. J Zool 249:181–186

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinheiro J, Bates D, DebRoy S, Sarkar D (2018) nlme: linear and nonlinear mixed effects models, https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=nlme

  • Podos J (2007) Discrimination of geographical song variants by Darwin’s finches. Anim Behav 73:833–844

    Google Scholar 

  • Podos J, Warren PS (2007) The evolution of geographic variation in birdsong. Adv Study Behav 37:403–458

    Google Scholar 

  • Price T (2008) Speciation in birds. Roberts and Company Publishers, Greenwood

    Google Scholar 

  • R Core Team (2018) R: a language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, http://www.R-project.org

  • Ramousse R, Le Berre M (1995) Pour un projet de charte de réintroduction de la Marmotte Alpine en France. In: Ramousse R, Le Berre M (eds) Laboratoire de Socioécologie et Conservation. International Marmot Network, Villeurbanne, pp 81–88

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramousse R, Martinot JP, Le Berre M (1992) Twenty years of re-introduction policy of alpine marmots from the national park of La Vanoise (French Alps). In: Bassano B, Durio P, Gallo Orsi U, Macchi E (eds) Proceedings of the 1st International Conference on Alpine Marmot (Marmota marmota) and on genus Marmota. Università degli Studi, Torino, pp 171–177

    Google Scholar 

  • Randler C, Förschler MI (2011) Heterospecifics do not respond to subtle differences in chaffinch mobbing calls: message is encoded in number of elements. Anim Behav 82:725–730

    Google Scholar 

  • Rendall D, Owren MJ, Ryan MJ (2009) What do animal signals mean? Anim Behav 78:233–240

    Google Scholar 

  • Richard JP (1991) Sound analysis and synthesis using an Amiga, micro-computer. Bioacoustics 3:45–60

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruegg K, Slabberkoorn H, Clegg S et al (2006) Divergence in mating signals correlates with ecological variation in the migratory songbird, Swainson’s thrush (Catharus ustulatus). Mol Ecol 15:3147–3156

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan MJ, Brenowitz EA (1985) The role of body size phylogeny and ambient noise in the evolution of bird song. Am Nat 126:87–100

    Google Scholar 

  • Scherrer JA, Wilkinson GS (1993) Evening bat isolation calls provide evidence for heritable signatures. Anim Behav 46:847–860

    Google Scholar 

  • Schlenker P, Chemla E, Arnold K, Lemasson A, Ouattara K, Keenan S, Stephan C, Ryder R, Zuberbühler K (2014) Monkey semantics: two ‘dialects’ of Campbell’s monkey alarm calls. Linguist Philos 37:439–501

    Google Scholar 

  • Searcy WA, Nowicki S, Hughes M, Peters S (2002) Geographic song discrimination in relation to dispersal distances in song sparrows. Am Nat 159:221–230

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shieh B-S, Liang S-H, Yuan H-W, Chen CC (2013) Experimental evidence that distinct song phrases in the Grey-cheeked Fulvetta Alcippe morrisonia permit species and local dialect recognition. Ibis 155:32–41

    Google Scholar 

  • Shizuka D, Lein MR, Chilton G (2016) Range-wide patterns of geographic variation in songs of Golden-crowned sparrows (Zonotrichia atricapilla). Auk 133:520–529

    Google Scholar 

  • Slabbekoorn H, Smith TB (2002) Bird song, ecology and speciation. Philos Trans R Soc B 357:493–503

    Google Scholar 

  • Slater PJB (1986) The cultural transmission of bird song. Trends Ecol Evol 1:94–97

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Slobodchikoff CN, Coast R (1980) Dialects in the alarm calls of prairie dogs. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 7:49–53

    Google Scholar 

  • Slobodchikoff CN, Ackers SH, Van Ert M (1998) Geographic variation in alarm calls of Gunnison’s prairie dogs. J Mammal 79:1265–1272

    Google Scholar 

  • Soha JA, Poesel A, Nelson DA, Lohr B (2016) Non-salient geographic variation in birdsong in a species that learns by improvisation. Ethology 122:343–353

    Google Scholar 

  • Staddon JER, McGeorge LW, Bruce RA et al (1978) A simple method for the rapid analysis of animal sounds. Z Tierpsychol 48:306–330

    Google Scholar 

  • Stoddard PK (1996) Vocal recognition of neighbours by territorial passerines. In: Kroodsma DE, Miller EH (eds) Ecology and evolution of acoustic communication in birds. Cornell University Press, Ithaca, pp 356–374

    Google Scholar 

  • Suzuki TN, Wheatcroft D, Griesser M (2016) Experimental evidence for compositional syntax in bird calls. Nat Commun 7:10986

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Thielcke G (2008) On the origin of divergence of learned signals (songs) in isolated populations. Ibis 115:511–516

    Google Scholar 

  • Thomas JA, Golladay CL (1992) Geographic variation in leopard seal (Hydrurga leptonyx) underwater vocalizations. In: Kastelijn RA, Thomas JA, Nachtigall PE (eds) Sensory systems of aquatic mammals. De Spil Publishers, Woerden, pp 201–221

    Google Scholar 

  • Thomas JA, Puddicombe RA, George M, Lewis D (1988) Variations in underwater vocalizations of Weddell seals (Leptonychotes weddelli) at the Vestfold Hills as a measure of breeding population discreteness. Hydrobiologia 165:279–284

    Google Scholar 

  • Tubaro PL, Mahler B (1998) Acoustic frequencies and body mass in New World doves. Condor 100:54–61

    Google Scholar 

  • Uy JAC, Moyle RG, Filardi CE (2009) Plumage and song differences mediate species recognition between incipient flycatcher species of the Solomon Islands. Evolution 63:153–164

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Parijs S, Lydersen C, Kovacs K (2003) Vocalizations and movements suggest alternative mating tactics in male bearded seals. Anim Behav 65:273–283

    Google Scholar 

  • Velásquez N (2014) Geographic variation in acoustic communication in anurans and its neuroethological implications. J Physiol 108:167–173

    Google Scholar 

  • Venables WN, Ripley BD (2002) Modern applied statistics with S, 4th edn. Springer, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Wei M, Lloyd H, Zhang Y (2011) Neighbour–stranger discrimination by Yellow-bellied Tit Parus venustulus: evidence for the “dear-enemy” effect. J Ornithol 152:431–438

    Google Scholar 

  • Weir BS, Cockerham CC (1984) Estimating F-statistics for the analysis of population-structure. Evolution 38:1358–1370

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wich SA, Krützen M, Lameira AR, Nater A, Arora N, Bastian ML, Meulman E, Morrogh-Bernard HC, Atmoko SSU, Pamungkas J, Perwitasari-Farajallah D, Hardus ME, van Noordwijk M, van Schaik CP (2012) Call cultures in orangutans? PLoS One 7:e36180

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wilczynski W, Ryan MJ (1999) Geographic variation in animal communication systems. In: Foster SA, Endler JA (eds) Variation in behavior: perspectives on evolutionary mechanisms. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 234–261

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiley RH, Richards DG (1978) Physical constraints on acoustic communication in the atmosphere: implications for the evolution of animal vocalizations. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 3:69–94

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiley RH, Richards DG (1982) Adaptations for acoustic communication in birds: sound transmission and signal detection. In: Kroodsma DE, Miller EH (eds) Acoustic communication in birds, vol 2. Academic Press, New York, pp 131–181

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilkins MR, Seddon N, Safran RJ (2013) Evolutionary divergence in acoustic signals: causes and consequences. Trends Ecol Evol 28:156–166

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman HD, Ramsay SM, Mesias V, Mora M, Murray BW, Otter KA (2016) Evolution of white-throated sparrow song: regional variation through shift in terminal strophe type and length. Behaviour 153:1839–1861

    Google Scholar 

  • Zuberbühler K (2009) Survivor signals: the biology and psychology of animal alarm calling. Adv Study Behav 40:277–322

    Google Scholar 

  • Zuk M, Rotenberry JT, Simmons LW (2001) Geographical variation in calling song of the field cricket Teleogryllus oceanicus: the importance of spatial scale. J Evol Biol 14:731–741

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank all the students involved in marmot recording. We are grateful to the golf of Tignes for allowing us to work on its green. We are deeply grateful to the commune of Tignes for the use of the Santel chalet. In the Pyrenees, we warmly thank all the people involved in field campaigns and special thanks to A. Planella and M. Unzeta (supported by a COLAB grant from the Catalan Government). MFR thanks the Generalitat de Catalunya (2017 SGR 1006). We further acknowledge James F. Hare, D. Blumstein, and two anonymous reviewers for helpful and constructive comments and suggestions that helped us to improve a previous version of this paper. We also thank F. Kirkpatrick Baird for carefully editing this paper.

Funding

This project was supported by COLAB and SGR (2017 SGR 1006) grants from the Catalan Government.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Thierry Lengagne or Mariona Ferrandiz-Rovira.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

All work adheres to the ASAB/ABS Guidelines for the Use of Animals in Research. The laboratory “Biométrie et Biologie Evolutive” is authorized to use animals in research (arrêté préfectoral no. DDPP69-2013-008), the protocol was approved by the University of Lyon 1 Ethical Committee (CEEA-55, protocol BH2012-92-V1), and the authorization to capture Alpine marmots was issued by the Préfecture de Savoie (arrêté préfectoral no. 2013/02) after approval by the advisory committee of the Nature Reserve of La Grande Sassière. Fieldwork was conducted under permit number AP n82010/121 by the Préfecture de la Savoie. All handling and sampling were done by three co-authors (MFR, AC, and IF) who are authorized for the experimentation with animals by the French Ministry of Agriculture and Fisheries or Catalan Government (diplomas 0ETRY20090520, R45GRETAF110, and 53707-UAB-FELASA).

Informed consent

Subjects gave their prior consent to participate in the study. To protect personal information, all data were collected anonymously.

Additional information

Communicated by T. Stankowich

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

The online version of this article contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

ESM 1

(PDF 198 kb)

ESM 2

(PDF 62 kb)

ESM 3

(PDF 243 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Lengagne, T., Ferrandiz-Rovira, M., Superbie, C. et al. Geographic variation in marmots’ alarm calls causes different responses. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 74, 97 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-020-02858-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-020-02858-5

Keywords

Navigation