Abstract
Developmental plasticity “prepares” individuals to environments experienced during adulthood. Labile traits, such as behaviour, vary within and among individuals owing to (i) reversible plasticity and (ii) developmental plasticity and genetic make-up, respectively. Here, we test whether developmental environments affect the expression of both within- and among-individual variation in behaviour. In ectothermic species, low temperatures are associated with a reduced expression of behaviour and can also limit the expression of reversible plasticity and among-individual differentiation, for example, by restricting the expression of additive genetic variation. We focused on exploratory behaviour since activity-related behaviours are assumed to be temperature-dependent in ectotherms. Specifically, low temperatures can restrict the physiological machinery needed for movement. To test our predictions, we raised field crickets, Gryllus bimaculatus, at low versus high temperatures throughout ontogeny and compared behavioural means and variance components (i.e. among- and within-individual variances) across treatments. We also compared the coefficients of variation for each variance component across treatments to assess whether environmental effects on variance were independent of the effects on mean behaviour. As predicted, individuals raised at high temperatures were more explorative and exhibited more among- and within-individual variance compared to individuals raised at low temperatures. Interestingly, individuals raised under high temperatures became more stable in their behaviour when controlling for treatment effects on average behaviour. Our results show experimentally that different developmental temperatures trigger different amounts of behavioural “stability” in exploratory behaviour, thereby suggesting a key role for temperature in affecting the ecological processes associated with exploratory behaviour in ectothermic species.
Significance statement
Individuals differ relative to one another in their mean behavioural expression (i.e. “animal personality”), while, at the same time, individuals change their behaviour from one instance to the next (i.e. reversible plasticity). Expression of both above-mentioned components should strongly depend on environments experienced throughout ontogeny. Temperature represents a key environmental factor in most ectothermic animals as it directly affects the ability to express behaviour. Here, we experimentally test whether temperature experienced throughout ontogeny has permanent effects on the expression of individual differences and reversible plasticity in behaviour later in life. We show that individuals become more stable, i.e. express less reversible plasticity, in risky exploratory behaviour when raised under high temperatures. Our results thus suggest a key role for temperature in affecting the ecological processes associated with risky behaviours in ectothermic species.
Similar content being viewed by others
References
Abram PK, Boivin G, Moiroux J, Brodeur J (2017) Behavioural effects of temperature on ectothermic animals: unifying thermal physiology and behavioural plasticity. Biol Rev 92:1859–1876. https://doi.org/10.1111/brv.12312
Auld JR, Agrawal AA, Relyea RA (2010) Re-evaluating the costs and limits of adaptive phenotypic plasticity. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 277:503–511. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2009.1355
Becher MA, Scharpenberg H, Moritz RFA (2009) Pupal developmental temperature and behavioral specialization of honeybee workers (Apis mellifera L.). J Comp Physiol A 195:673–679. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00359-009-0442-7
Bégin M, Roff DA, Debat V (2004) The effect of temperature and wing morphology on quantitative genetic variation in the cricket Gryllus firmus, with an appendix examining the statistical properties of the Jackknife-MANOVA method of matrix comparison. J Evol Biol 17:1255–1267. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1420-9101.2004.00772.x
Bengston SE, Pruitt JN, Riechert SE (2014) Differences in environmental enrichment generate contrasting behavioural syndromes in a basal spider lineage. Anim Behav 93:105–110. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.04.022
Biro PA, Adriaenssens B (2013) Predictability as a personality trait: consistent differences in intraindividual behavioral variation. Am Nat 182:621–629. https://doi.org/10.1086/673213
Bolnick DI, Amarasekare P, Araújo MS, Bürger R, Levine JM, Novak M, Rudolf VHW, Schreiber SJ, Urban MC, Vasseur DA (2011) Why intraspecific trait variation matters in community ecology. Trends Ecol Evol 26:183–192
Briffa M (2013) Plastic proteans: reduced predictability in the face of predation risk in hermit crabs. Biol Lett 9:20130592–20130592. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2013.0592
Briffa M, Bridger D, Biro PA (2013) How does temperature affect behaviour? Multilevel analysis of plasticity, personality and predictability in hermit crabs. Anim Behav 86:47–54
Butler MW, Toomey MB, McGraw KJ, Rowe M (2012) Ontogenetic immune challenges shape adult personality in mallard ducks. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 279:326–333. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2011.0842
Campbell GS, Norman JM (1998) An introduction to environmental biophysics. Springer, New York
Careau V, Buttemer WA, Buchanan KL (2014) Early-developmental stress, repeatability, and canalization in a suite of physiological and behavioral traits in female zebra finches. Integr Comp Biol 54:539–554. https://doi.org/10.1093/icb/icu095
Chapman BB, Hulthén K, Blomqvist DR, Hansson LA, Nilsson JÅ, Brodersen J, Anders Nilsson P, Skov C, Brönmark C (2011) To boldly go: individual differences in boldness influence migratory tendency. Ecol Lett 14:871–876. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1461-0248.2011.01648.x
Charmantier A, Garant D (2005) Environmental quality and evolutionary potential: lessons from wild populations. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 272:1415–1425. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2005.3117
Cleasby IR, Nakagawa S, Schielzeth H (2015) Quantifying the predictability of behaviour: statistical approaches for the study of between-individual variation in the within-individual variance. Methods Ecol Evol 6:27–37. https://doi.org/10.1111/2041-210X.12281
core team R (2014) R: a language and environment for statistical computing. R Found Stat Comput, Vienna
D’Amore DM, Rios-Cardenas O, Morris MR (2015) Maternal investment influences development of behavioural syndrome in swordtail fish, Xiphophorus multilineatus. Anim Behav 103:147–151. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.02.013
Dall SRX, Bell AM, Bolnick DI, Ratnieks FLW (2012) An evolutionary ecology of individual differences. Ecol Lett 15:1189–1198. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1461-0248.2012.01846.x
Dammhahn M, Dingemanse NJ, Niemelä PT, Réale D (2018) Pace-of-life syndromes: a framework for the adaptive integration of behaviour, physiology and life history. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 72:62. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-018-2473-y
DeWitt TJ, Sih A, Wilson DS (1998) Costs and limits of phenotypic plasticity. Trends Ecol Evol 13:77–81. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0169-5347(97)01274-3
Dingemanse NJ, Dochtermann NA (2013) Quantifying individual variation in behaviour: mixed-effect modelling approaches. J Anim Ecol 82:39–54. https://doi.org/10.1111/1365-2656.12013
Dingemanse NJ, Bouwman KM, van de Pol M, van Overveld T, Patrick SC, Matthysen E, Quinn JL (2012) Variation in personality and behavioural plasticity across four populations of the great tit Parus major. J Anim Ecol 81:116–126. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2656.2011.01877.x
DiRienzo N, Montiglio PO (2016) The contribution of developmental experience vs. condition to life history, trait variation and individual differences. J Anim Ecol 85:915–926
DiRienzo N, Pruitt JN, Hedrick AV (2012) Juvenile exposure to acoustic sexual signals from conspecifics alters growth trajectory and an adult personality trait. Anim Behav 84:861–868. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.07.007
DiRienzo N, Niemelä PT, Skog A et al (2015) Juvenile pathogen exposure affects the presence of personality in adult field crickets. Front Ecol Evol 3. https://doi.org/10.3389/fevo.2015.00036
Doherty J (1985) Temperature coupling and “trade-off” phenomena in the acoustic communication system of the cricket, Gryllus bimaculatus de Geer (Gryllidae). J Exp Biol 114:17–35
Duckworth RA (2009) The role of behavior in evolution: a search for mechanism. Evol Ecol 23:513–531. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10682-008-9252-6
Falconer DS, Mackay TFC (1996) Introduction to quantitative genetics, Ed 4. Longmans Green, Harlow, Essex, UK
Fraser DF, Gilliam JF, Daley MJ, le AN, Skalski GT (2001) Explaining leptokurtic movement distributions: intrapopulation variation in boldness and exploration. Am Nat 158:124–135. https://doi.org/10.1086/321307
Gasparini G, Lu C, Dingemanse NJ, Tuni C (2018) Paternal-effects in a terrestrial ectotherm are temperature dependent but no evidence for adaptive effects. Funct Ecol 32:1011-1021.
Gracceva G, Koolhaas JM, Groothuis TGG (2011) Does the early social environment affect structure and consistency of personality in wild-type male’s rat? Dev Psychobiol 53:614–623. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.20586
Guenther A, Finkemeier MA, Trillmich F (2014) The ontogeny of personality in the wild guinea pig. Anim Behav 90:131–139. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.01.032
Hadfield JD (2010) MCMC methods for multi-response generalized linear mixed models: the MCMCglmm R package. J Stat Softw 33:1–22. https://doi.org/10.1002/ana.22635
Han CS, Dingemanse NJ (2017) You are what you eat: diet shapes body composition, personality and behavioural stability. BMC Evol Biol 17:8. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12862-016-0852-4
Hansen TF, Pélabon C, Houle D (2011) Heritability is not evolvability. Evol Biol 38:258–277. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11692-011-9127-6
Hedrick AV, Perez D, Lichti N, Yew J (2002) Temperature preferences of male field crickets (Gryllus integer) alter their mating calls. J Comp Physiol A 188:799–805. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00359-002-0368-9
Hoffmann AA, Merilä J (1999) Heritable variation and evolution under favourable and unfavourable conditions. Trends Ecol Evol 14:96–101. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0169-5347(99)01595-5
Houle D (1992) Comparing evolvability and variability of quantitative traits. Genetics 130:195–204 doi: citeulike-article-id:10041224
Houle D, Pélabon C, Wagner GP, Hansen TF (2011) Measurement and meaning in biology. Q Rev Biol 86:3–34. https://doi.org/10.1086/658408
Lachenicht MW, Clusella-Trullas S, Boardman L, le Roux C, Terblanche JS (2010) Effects of acclimation temperature on thermal tolerance, locomotion performance and respiratory metabolism in Acheta domesticus L. (Orthoptera: Gryllidae). J Insect Physiol 56:822–830. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jinsphys.2010.02.010
Laskowski KL, Pruitt JN (2014) Evidence of social niche construction: persistent and repeated social interactions generate stronger personalities in a social spider. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 281:20133166–20133166. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2013.3166
Lehmann FO (1999) Ambient temperature affects free-flight performance in the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster. J Comp Physiol B 169:165–171. https://doi.org/10.1007/s003600050207
Lenth R (2016) lsmeans: least-squares means. R-package version 220-23. J. Stat. Software 69:1-33. doi: http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lsmeans
Liedtke J, Redekop D, Schneider JM, Schuett W (2015) Early environmental conditions shape personality types in a jumping spider. Front Ecol Evol 3. https://doi.org/10.3389/fevo.2015.00134
Lima SL (1998) Nonlethal effects in the ecology of predator-prey interactions. Bioscience 48:25–34. https://doi.org/10.2307/1313225
Lima SL, Dill LM (1990) Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation: a review and prospectus. Can J Zool 68:619–640. https://doi.org/10.1139/z90-092
Lynch M, Walsh B (1998) Genetics and analysis of quantitative traits. Sinauer, Sunderland, MA
McGhee KE, Travis J (2011) Early food and social environment affect certain behaviours but not female choice or male dominance in bluefin killifish. Anim Behav 82:139–147. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.04.009
Montiglio PO, Royauté R (2014) Contaminants as a neglected source of behavioural variation. Anim Behav 88:29–25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.11.018
Naguib M, Flörcke C, Van Oers K (2011) Effects of social conditions during early development on stress response and personality traits in great tits (Parus major). Dev Psychobiol 53:592–600. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.20533
Niemelä PT, Dingemanse NJ (2017) Individual versus pseudo-repeatability in behaviour: lessons from translocation experiments in a wild insect. J Anim Ecol 86:1033–1043. https://doi.org/10.1111/1365-2656.12688
Niemelä PT, Dingemanse NJ (2018) Meta-analysis reveals weak associations between intrinsic state and personality. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 285:20172823. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2017.2823
Niemelä PT, Vainikka A, Lahdenperä S, Kortet R (2012) Nymphal density, behavioral development, and life history in a field cricket. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 66:645–652. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-011-1312-1
Niemelä PT, Lattenkamp EZ, Dingemanse NJ (2015) Personality-related survival and sampling bias in wild cricket nymphs. Behav Ecol 26:936–946. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arv036
Nussey DH, Wilson AJ, Brommer JE (2007) The evolutionary ecology of individual phenotypic plasticity in wild populations. J Evol Biol 20:831–844. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1420-9101.2007.01300.x
Nylin S, Gotthard K (1998) Plasticity in life-history traits. Annu Rev Entomol 43:63–83. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ento.43.1.63
Pickett STA, Cadenasso ML (1995) Landscape ecology: spatial heterogeneity in ecological systems. Science 80(269):331–334. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.269.5222.331
Piersma T, Drent J (2003) Phenotypic flexibility and the evolution of organismal design. Trends Ecol Evol 18:228–233. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0169-5347(03)00036-3
Pires A, Hoy RR (1992a) Temperature coupling in cricket acoustic communication - I. Field and laboratory studies of temperature effects on calling song production and recognition in Gryllus firmus. J Comp Physiol A 171:69–78. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00195962
Pires A, Hoy RR (1992b) Temperature coupling in cricket acoustic communication - II. Localization of temperature effects on song production and recognition networks in Gryllus firmus. J Comp Physiol A 171:79–92. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00195963
Réale D, Reader SM, Sol D, McDougall PT, Dingemanse NJ (2007) Integrating animal temperament within ecology and evolution. Biol Rev 82:291–318. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-185X.2007.00010.x
Royauté R, Dochtermann NA (2017) When the mean no longer matters: developmental diet affects behavioral variation but not population averages in the house cricket (Acheta domesticus). Behav Ecol 28:337–345. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arw164
Royauté R, Buddle CM, Vincent C (2015) Under the influence: sublethal exposure to an insecticide affects personality expression in a jumping spider. Funct Ecol 29:962–970. https://doi.org/10.1111/1365-2435.12413
Ruuskanen S, Laaksonen T (2010) Yolk hormones have sex-specific long-term effects on behavior in the pied flycatcher (Ficedula hypoleuca). Horm Behav 57:119–127. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.09.017
Santostefano F, Wilson AJ, Araya-Ajoy YI, Dingemanse NJ (2016) Interacting with the enemy: indirect effects of personality on conspecific aggression in crickets. Behav Ecol 27:1235–1246. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arw037
Santostefano F, Wilson AJ, Niemelä PT, Dingemanse NJ (2017a) Behavioural mediators of genetic life-history trade-offs in field crickets. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 284:20171567.
Santostefano F, Wilson AJ, Niemelä PT, Dingemanse NJ (2017b) Indirect genetic effects: a key component of the genetic architecture of behaviour. Sci Rep 7:10235. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-08258-6
Sinn DL, Gosling SD, Moltschaniwskyj NA (2008) Development of shy/bold behaviour in squid: context-specific phenotypes associated with developmental plasticity. Anim Behav 75:433–442
Snell-Rood EC (2013) An overview of the evolutionary causes and consequences of behavioural plasticity. Anim Behav 85:1004–1011. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.12.031
Stamps J, Groothuis TGG (2010) The development of animal personality: relevance, concepts and perspectives. Biol Rev 85:301–325. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-185X.2009.00103.x
Stamps JA, Briffa M, Biro PA (2012) Unpredictable animals: individual differences in intraindividual variability (IIV). Anim Behav 83:1325–1334. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.02.017
Taylor LR (1961) Aggregation, variance and the mean. Nature 189:732–735. https://doi.org/10.1038/189732a0
Tremmel M, Müller C (2013) Insect personality depends on environmental conditions. Behav Ecol 24:386–392. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/ars175
Tuomainen U, Candolin U (2011) Behavioural responses to human-induced environmental change. Biol Rev 86:640–657. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-185X.2010.00164.x
Urszán TJ, Garamszegi LZ, Nagy G, Hettyey A, Török J, Herczeg G (2015) No personality without experience? A test on Rana dalmatina tadpoles. Ecol Evol 5(24):5847–5856. https://doi.org/10.1002/ece3.1804
Vedder O, Bouwhuis S, Sheldon BC (2013) Quantitative assessment of the importance of phenotypic plasticity in adaptation to climate change in wild bird populations. PLoS Biol 11. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1001605
West-Eberhardt MJ (2003) Developmental plasticity and evolution. Oxford University Press, New York
Westneat DF, Wright J, Dingemanse NJ (2015) The biology hidden inside residual within-individual phenotypic variation. Biol Rev 90:729–743. https://doi.org/10.1111/brv.12131
Whitman DW (2009) The significance of body size in the orthoptera: a review. J. Orthop. Res. 17:117–134. https://doi.org/10.1665/1082-6467-17.2.117
Wolf M, Weissing FJ (2012) Animal personalities: consequences for ecology and evolution. Trends Ecol Evol 27:452–461
Acknowledgements
We thank two anonymous referees for constructive comments for the manuscript.
Funding
P.T.N. was funded by Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG grant no. NI 1539/1-1); CT was funded by the BioNa Junior Scientist Award of the Ludwig-Maximilians University; C.G. was funded by a Research Collaboration Award from UWA and an ARC DECRA (DE150101625) fellowship; and P.N. and N.J.D were funded by the Ludwig-Maximilians University.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Additional information
Communicated by K. Shaw
Publisher’s note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Niemelä, P.T., Niehoff, P., Gasparini, C. et al. Crickets become behaviourally more stable when raised under higher temperatures. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 73, 81 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-019-2689-5
Received:
Revised:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-019-2689-5