Aging male loons make a terminal investment in territory defense

Abstract

Animals that senesce experience a decline in residual reproductive value (RRV), such that old individuals can expect reduced breeding success compared to young ones. According to life history theory, animals with low RRV, which thus have less to lose, should shift resources away from self-maintenance and towards reproduction, an expectation called terminal investment. In a population of common loons whose survival and territorial behavior were measured throughout life, males 14 years and older exhibited clear senescence, as they showed lower survival, reduced body mass, and far greater susceptibility to territorial eviction than younger males. While older males invested no more effort than young males in feeding or protecting their chicks, they increased territorial yodeling by 35%, showed more aggression towards territorial intruders, and, following eviction from original territories, resettled with great frequency on vacant, unproductive territories nearby. Our findings thus provide support for terminal investment in territorial behavior. Hyper-aggressive behavior by old, declining males might explain the unusual occurrence of lethal combat for territories in this species.

Significance statement

Life history theory holds that animals that decline with age should invest less energy in staying alive and more in reproduction when they become old, because they have less to lose in doing so. Evidence for such terminal investment, however, is scanty. We measured rates of survival and territorial behavior in a known-age population of common loons and found that males, but not females, suffered a decrease in survival rate and ability to hold a territory after age 14. At the same time, territorial males older than 14 behaved more aggressively and were more apt to give a territorial yodel call towards territorial intruders. Hence, our study appears a rare example of terminal investment in aggressive behavior, whereby old, declining males cling desperately to their territories in hopes of producing a few more offspring before they die.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

References

  1. Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. Behaviour 49:227–267

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  2. Berube CH, Festa-Bianchet M, Jorgenson JT (1999) Individual differences, longevity, and reproductive senescence in bighorn ewes. Ecology 80:2555–2565

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Biro PA, Stamps JA (2010) Do consistent individual differences in metabolic rate promote consistent individual differences in behavior? Trends Ecol Evol 25:653–659

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Bouwhuis S, Choquet R, Sheldon BC, Verhulst S (2012) The forms and fitness cost of senescence: age-specific recapture, survival, reproduction, and reproductive value in a wild bird population. Am Nat 179:E15–E27

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Bowers EK, Bowden RM, Sakaluk SK, Thompson CF (2015) Immune activation generates corticosterone-mediated terminal reproductive investment in a wild bird. Am Nat 185:769–783

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Briggs CW, Woodbridge B, Collopy MW (2011) Correlates of survival in Swainson’s hawks breeding in northern California. J Wildl Manag 75:1307–1314

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Burnham K, Anderson D (2002) Model selection and multimodel inference. Springer, New York

    Google Scholar 

  8. Clutton-Brock TH (1984) Reproductive effort and terminal investment in iteroparous animals. Am Nat 123:212–229

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Clutton-Brock TH, Isvaran K (2007) Sex differences in ageing in natural populations of vertebrates. Proc R Soc Lond B 274:3097–3104

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Cooch E, White G (2006) Program MARK: a gentle introduction, http://www.phidot.org/software/mark/docs/book

  11. Descamps S, Boutin S, Berteaux D, Gaillard JM (2007) Female red squirrels fit Williams’ hypothesis of increasing reproductive effort with increasing age. J Anim Ecol 76:1192–1201

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Duffield K, Hunt J, Rapkin J, Sadd B, Sakaluk S (2015) Terminal investment in the gustatory appeal of nuptial food gifts in crickets. J Evol Biol 28:1872–1881

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Evers DC, Paruk JD, Mcintyre JW, Barr JF (2010) Common loon (Gavia immer). In: The birds of North America. Cornell Lab of Ornithology, Ithaca, http://bna.birds.cornell.edu.proxy2.lib.uwo.ca:2048

  14. Felton A, Alford RA, Felton AM, Schwarzkopf L (2006) Multiple mate choice criteria and the importance of age for male mating success in the microhylid frog, Cophixalus ornatus. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 59:786–795

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Fife D, Pollet I, Robertson G, Mallory M, Shutler D (2015) Apparent survival of adult leach’s storm-petrels (Oceanodroma leucorhoa) breeding on Bon Portage Island, Nova Scotia. Avian Conserv Ecol 10:1

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Fisher RA (1930) The genetical theory of natural selection: a complete variorum edition. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  17. Froy H, Phillips RA, Wood AG, Nussey DH, Lewis S (2013) Age-related variation in reproductive traits in the wandering albatross: evidence for terminal improvement following senescence. Ecol Lett 16:642–649

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. González-Tokman DM, González-Santoyo I, Córdoba-Aguilar A (2013) Mating success and energetic condition effects driven by terminal investment in territorial males of a short-lived invertebrate. Funct Ecol 27:739–747

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Hall ML, Molles LE, Illes AE, Vehrencamp SL (2009) Singing in the face of death: male banded wrens Thryophilus pleurostictus sing more to playback in their last breeding season. J Avian Biol 40:217–224

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Hamilton WD (1966) The moulding of senescence by natural selection. J Theor Biol 12:12–45

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  21. Hayward AD, Wilson AJ, Pilkington JG, Clutton-Brock TH, Pemberton JM, Kruuk LE (2013) Reproductive senescence in female Soay sheep: variation across traits and contributions of individual ageing and selective disappearance. Funct Ecol 27:184–195

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Hyman J, Hughes M, Searcy WA, Nowicki S (2004) Individual variation in the strength of territory defense in male song sparrows: correlates of age, territory tenure, and neighbor aggressiveness. Behaviour 141:15–27

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Itoh Y, Suzuki M, Ogawa A, Munechika I, Murata K, Mizuno S (2001) Identification of the sex of a wide range of Carinatae birds by PCR using primer sets selected from chicken EE0. 6 and its related sequences. J Hered 92:315–321

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  24. Jukkala G, Piper W (2015) Common loon parents defend chicks according to both value and vulnerability. J Avian Biol 46:551–558

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Kuczynski MC, Bello-DeOcampo D, Getty T (2015) No evidence of terminal investment in the gray treefrog (Hyla versicolor): older males do not signal at greater effort. Copeia 103:530–535

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Liker A, Szekely T (2005) Mortality costs of sexual selection and parental care in natural populations of birds. Evolution 59:890–897

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Mager JN, Walcott C, Piper WH (2007) Male common loons, Gavia immer, communicate body mass and condition through dominant frequencies of territorial yodels. Anim Behav 73:683–690

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Mager JN, Walcott C, Piper WH (2012) Male common loons signal greater aggressive motivation by lengthening territorial yodels. Wilson J Ornithol 124:73–80

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Møller AP, Nielsen JT (2014) Parental defense of offspring and life history of a long-lived raptor. Behav Ecol 25:1505–1512

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Newton I, Rothery P (1997) Senescence and reproductive value in sparrowhawks. Ecology 78:1000–1008

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Nussey DH, Froy H, Lemaitre J-F, Gaillard J-M, Austad SN (2013) Senescence in natural populations of animals: widespread evidence and its implications for bio-gerontology. Ageing Res Rev 12:214–225

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Ory NC, van Son TC, Thiel M (2015) Mating rock shrimp hedge their bets: old males take greater risk, but only after careful assessment of the investment scenario. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 69:1975–1984

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Owens I, Hartley I (1998) Sexual dimorphism in birds: why are there so many different forms of dimorphism? Proc R Soc Lond B 265:397–407

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Part T, Gustafsson L, Moreno J (1992) “Terminal investment” and a sexual conflict in the collared flycatcher (Ficedula albicollis). Am Nat 140:868–882

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  35. Pianka ER, Parker WS (1975) Age-specific reproductive tactics. Am Nat 109:453–464

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Piper WH, Brunk KM, Flory JA, Meyer MW (2017) The long shadow of senescence: age impacts survival and territory defense in loons. J Avian Biol (published online, https://doi.org/10.1111/jav.01393)

  37. Piper WH, Grear JS, Meyer MW (2012) Juvenile survival in common loons Gavia immer: effects of natal lake size and pH. J Avian Biol 43:280–288

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Piper WH, Mager JN, Walcott C, Furey L, Banfield N, Reinke A, Spilker F, Flory JA (2015) Territory settlement in common loons: no footholds but age and assessment are important. Anim Behav 104:155–163

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Piper WH, Palmer MW, Banfield N, Meyer MW (2013) Can settlement in natal-like habitat explain maladaptive habitat selection? Proc R Soc B 280:20130979

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Piper WH, Tischler KB, Klich M (2000) Territory acquisition in loons: the importance of take-over. Anim Behav 59:385–394

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  41. Piper WH, Walcott C, Mager JN III, Perala M, Tischler KB, Harrington E, Turcotte AJ, Schwabenlander M, Banfield N (2006) Prospecting in a solitary breeder: chick production elicits territorial intrusions in common loons. Behav Ecol 17:881–888

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Piper WH, Walcott C, Mager JN, Spilker FJ (2008a) Fatal battles in common loons: a preliminary analysis. Anim Behav 75:1109–1115

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Piper WH, Walcott C, Mager JN, Spilker FJ (2008b) Nestsite selection by male loons leads to sex-biased site familiarity. J Anim Ecol 77:205–210

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Promislow DE, Montgomerie R, Martin TE (1992) Mortality costs of sexual dimorphism in birds. Proc R Soc Lond B 250:143–150

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Pugesek BH (1981) Increased reproductive effort with age in the California gull (Larus californicus). Science 212:822–823

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  46. Ricklefs RE (1998) Evolutionary theories of aging: confirmation of a fundamental prediction, with implications for the genetic basis and evolution of life span. Am Nat 152:24–44

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  47. Sergio F, Tavecchia G, Blas J, López L, Tanferna A, Hiraldo F (2011) Variation in age-structured vital rates of a long-lived raptor: implications for population growth. Basic Appl Ecol 12:107–115

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Waas JR (1991) The risks and benefits of signalling aggressive motivation: a study of cave-dwelling little blue penguins. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 29:139–146

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Walcott C, Evers D, Froehler M, Krakauer A (1999) Individuality in “yodel” calls recorded from a banded population of common loons, Gavia immer. Bioacoustics 10:101–114

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Weil ZM, Martin LB, Workman JL, Nelson RJ (2006) Immune challenge retards seasonal reproductive regression in rodents: evidence for terminal investment. Biol Lett 2:393–396

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. White GC, Burnham KP (1999) Program MARK: survival estimation from populations of marked animals. Bird Study 46:S120–S139

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank three collaborators, Charles Walcott, Jay Mager, and Joel Flory, and many research assistants, especially Andrew Reinke, Nathan Banfield, Keren Tischler, Margaret Klich, Frank Spilker, Lyla Furey, Erin Harrington, and Amy Turcotte. We also thank two anonymous reviewers.

Funding

The research was supported by the National Science Foundation (IBN-0316442 and DEB-0717055), the National Geographic Society, the Disney Conservation Fund, and Chapman University.

Author information

Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Walter H. Piper.

Ethics declarations

Ethical approval

We followed all applicable international, national, and institutional guidelines for the care and use of animals. All protocols for capture, marking and study of common loons were approved by the Chapman University Animal Care and Use Committee.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Communicated by C. R. Brown

Rights and permissions

Reprints and Permissions

About this article

Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Piper, W.H., Brunk, K.M., Jukkala, G.L. et al. Aging male loons make a terminal investment in territory defense. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 72, 95 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-018-2511-9

Download citation

Keywords

  • Terminal investment
  • Aggression
  • Territory
  • Loon
  • Gavia immer