Advertisement

Social lateralization in wild Asian elephants: visual preferences of mothers and offspring

  • Karina KareninaEmail author
  • Andrey Giljov
  • Shermin de Silva
  • Yegor Malashichev
Original Article

Abstract

Theoretical and empirical evidence suggest that socio-biological factors determine the expression of behavioural lateralization across species. One would expect the same association at the intraspecific level, that is, that the differences in social strategies of the two sexes entail the sex differences in the lateralized social processing. This study aimed to test whether this hypothesis applies to the lateralized behaviour of offspring towards a mother. The preferences in the use of the lateral visual field of the left and right eye were assessed in wild Asian elephant, Elephas maximus mothers and their young sons and daughters. The spatial positioning relative to a social partner during approach was used as a behavioural indicator of visual lateralization. At the population level, elephant mothers preferred to keep the young in their left visual field during slow travelling. In contrast, young did not display a one-sided bias for the whole sample. The lateralization, however, was pronounced in a sex-specific manner—sons preferentially kept their mothers in the right visual field, while daughters preferred to keep mothers in the left visual field. Intriguingly, both sons and daughters preferentially kept the familiar older young in the left visual field. Sons, thus, showed oppositely directed lateral preferences towards mother and non-mother companion. Presumably, sons aim to approach the mother from her left side (rather than to keep her in the right visual field) and benefit from optimized maternal perception, while daughters facilitate their own perception of the mother by keeping her in the left visual field. These sex-related differences in lateralized behaviour may result from strikingly different social strategies of two sexes.

Significance statement

Young mammals show robust lateralization in the form of one-sided behavioural preferences in the interactions with their mother. Previous studies suggest that the social lifestyle may serve as a driving force in the evolution of behavioural lateralization. To test this proposition, we investigated behavioural lateralization in young subjects of Asian elephants, a species in which females are more gregarious than males. The lateralized behaviour of offspring towards the mother was found to be strongly sex-specific. In contrast, interactions with older young were lateralized in a similar manner in sons and daughters. Our results suggest that the benefits of a left-sided or right-sided position relative to mother have different significance for sons and daughters because of the distinctive social strategies of two sexes.

Keywords

Mother–infant laterality Lateralized social response Lateralized behaviour Maternal behaviour Sex differences in lateralization Laterality in elephants 

Notes

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to the field staff of the Uda Walawe Elephant Research Project and Uda Walawe National Park personnel. The authors thank the editor (Bernhard Voelkl) and anonymous reviewers for their helpful suggestions and comments on the manuscript.

Authors’ contributions

KK and AG designed and organized the study, performed data collection and analyses and wrote the paper. SdS organized the field work and edited the paper. YM supervised the project and contributed to and edited the paper.

Funding

The complex zoological expedition to Sri Lanka was funded by Saint Petersburg University (grant no. 1.42.1095.2016). The analytical work was conducted within a broader framework of the project funded by the Russian Science Foundation (no. 14-14-00284).

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

This study followed the ASAB/ABS (2012) guidelines for the use of animals in research and was approved by St. Petersburg State University ethical committee (permit no. 131-03-5).

Supplementary material

265_2018_2440_MOESM1_ESM.pdf (16 kb)
ESM 1 Output of logistic regression model used to analyze possible associations of the distribution of lateralized and non-lateralized individuals (dependent variable) with age and sex (independent variable). (PDF 16 kb)

References

  1. Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. Behaviour 49(3):227–266.  https://doi.org/10.1163/156853974X00534 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Arivazhagan C, Sukumar R (2008) Constructing age structures of Asian elephant populations: a comparison of two field methods of age estimation. Gajah 29:11–16Google Scholar
  3. Austin NP, Rogers LJ (2012) Limb preferences and lateralization of aggression, reactivity and vigilance in feral horses, Equus caballus. Anim Behav 83(1):239–247.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.10.033 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Baraud I, Buytet B, Bec P, Blois-Heulin C (2009) Social laterality and ‘transversality’ in two species of mangabeys: influence of rank and implication for hemispheric specialization. Behav Brain Res 198(2):449–458.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2008.11.032 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Benelli G, Romano D, Messing RH, Canale A (2015) Population-level lateralized aggressive and courtship displays make better fighters not lovers: evidence from a fly. Behav Process 115:163–168.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2015.04.005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bisazza A, Cantalupo C, Capocchiano M, Vallortigara G (2000) Population lateralisation and social behaviour: a study with 16 species of fish. Laterality 5(3):269–284.  https://doi.org/10.1080/713754381 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Boeving ER, Belnap SC, Nelson EL (2017) Embraces are lateralized in spider monkeys (Ateles fusciceps rufiventris). Am J Primatol 79(6):e22654.  https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.22654 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bourne VJ, Todd BK (2004) When left means right: an explanation of the left cradling bias in terms of right hemisphere specializations. Dev Sci 7(1):19–24.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00318.x CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Brancucci A, Lucci G, Mazzatenta A, Tommasi L (2009) Asymmetries of the human social brain in the visual, auditory and chemical modalities. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 364(1519):895–914.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0279 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Calvo MG, Rodríguez-Chinea S, Fernández-Martín A (2015) Lateralized discrimination of emotional scenes in peripheral vision. Exp Brain Res 233(3):997–1006.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-014-4174-8 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Chapelain A, Pimbert P, Aube L, Perrocheau O, Debunne G, Bellido A, Blois-Heulin C (2015) Can population-level laterality stem from social pressures? Evidence from cheek kissing in humans. PLoS One 10(8):e0124477.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0124477 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  12. Chivers DP, McCormick MI, Allan BJ, Mitchell MD, Gonçalves EJ, Bryshun R, Ferrari MC (2016) At odds with the group: changes in lateralization and escape performance reveal conformity and conflict in fish schools. Proc R Soc B 283(1841):20161127.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2016.1127 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Coren S (1993) Measurement of handedness via self-report: the relationship between brief and extended inventories. Percept Mot Skills 76(3):1035–1042.  https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1993.76.3.1035 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Davidson RJ (2004) Well-being and affective style: neural substrates and biobehavioural correlates. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 359(1449):1395–1411.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2004.1510 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  15. de Silva S, Ranjeewa AD, Kryazhimskiy S (2011) The dynamics of social networks among female Asian elephants. BMC Ecol 11(1):17.  https://doi.org/10.1186/1472-6785-11-17 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  16. de Silva S, Ranjeewa AD, Weerakoon D (2011) Demography of Asian elephants (Elephas maximus) at Uda Walawe National Park, Sri Lanka based on identified individuals. Biol Conserv 144(5):1742–1752.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biocon.2011.03.011 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Deng C, Rogers LJ (2002) Social recognition and approach in the chick: lateralization and effect of visual experience. Anim Behav 63(4):697–706.  https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2001.1942 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Forrester GS, Crawley M, Palmer C (2014) Social environment elicits lateralized navigational paths in two populations of typically developing children. Brain Cogn 91:21–27.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2014.07.005 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Fowler ME, Mikota SK (2008) Biology, medicine, and surgery of elephants. Blackwell, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  20. Frasnelli E (2013) Brain and behavioral lateralization in invertebrates. Front Psychol 4:939CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  21. Ghirlanda S, Frasnelli E, Vallortigara G (2009) Intraspecific competition and coordination in the evolution of lateralization. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 364(1519):861–866.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0227 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Ghirlanda S, Vallortigara G (2004) The evolution of brain lateralization: a game-theoretical analysis of population structure. Proc R Soc Lond B 271(1541):853–858.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2003.2669 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Giljov A, Karenina K, de Silva S (2017) Context-dependent lateralization of trunk movements in wild Asian elephants. Biol Commun 62(2):82–92.  https://doi.org/10.21638/11701/spbu03.2017.204 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Güven M, Elalmis DD, Binokay S, Tan U (2003) Population-level right-paw preference in rats assessed by a new computerized food-reaching test. Int J Neurosci 113(12):1675–1689.  https://doi.org/10.1080/00207450390249258 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Harris LJ (2010) Side biases for holding and carrying infants: reports from the past and possible lessons for today. Laterality 15(1-2):56–135.  https://doi.org/10.1080/13576500802584371 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Hausmann M (2017) Why sex hormones matter for neuroscience: a very short review on sex, sex hormones, and functional brain asymmetries. J Neurosci Res 95(1-2):40–49.  https://doi.org/10.1002/jnr.23857 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Hecht D (2014) Cerebral lateralization of pro-and anti-social tendencies. Exp Neurobiol 23(1):1–27.  https://doi.org/10.5607/en.2014.23.1.1 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  28. Herlitz A, Nilsson L-G, Backman L (1997) Gender differences in episodic memory. Mem Cognition 25(6):801–811.  https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03211324 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Hopkins WD (2004) Laterality in maternal cradling and infant positional biases: implications for the development and evolution of hand preferences in nonhuman primates. Int J Primatol 25(6):1243–1265.  https://doi.org/10.1023/B:IJOP.0000043961.89133.3d CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  30. Huggenberger HJ, Suter SE, Reijnen E, Schachinger H (2009) Cradling side preference is associated with lateralized processing of baby facial expressions in females. Brain Cogn 70(1):67–72.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2008.12.010 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. James TW, Kimura D (1997) Sex differences in remembering the locations of objects in an array: location-shift versus location-exchanges. Evol Hum Behav 18(3):155–163.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S1090-5138(97)00004-4 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Karenina K, Giljov A, Glazov D, Malashichev Y (2013) Social laterality in wild beluga whale infants: comparisons between locations, escort conditions, and ages. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 67(7):1195–1204.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-013-1545-2 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Karenina K, Giljov A, Ingram J, Rowntree VJ, Malashichev Y (2017) Lateralization of mother–infant interactions in a diverse range of mammal species. Nat Ecol Evol 1(2):0030.  https://doi.org/10.1038/s41559-016-0030 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Karenina K, Giljov A, Ivkovich T, Burdin A, Malashichev Y (2013) Lateralization of spatial relationships between wild mother and infant orcas, Orcinus orca. Anim Behav 86(6):1225–1231.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.09.025 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Lee PC, Moss CJ (1986) Early maternal investment in male and female African elephant calves. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18(5):353–361.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00299666 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Lee PC, Moss CJ (2011) Calf development and maternal rearing strategies. In: Moss CJ, Croze H, Lee PC (eds) The Amboseli elephants: a long-term perspective on a long-lived mammal. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 224–237.  https://doi.org/10.7208/chicago/9780226542263.003.0014 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Lindell AK (2013) Continuities in emotion lateralization in human and nonhuman primates. Front Hum Neurosci 7:464CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  38. Lorberbaum JP, Newman JD, Horwitz AR, Dubno JR, Lydiard RB, Hamner MB, Bohning DE, George MS (2002) A potential role for thalamocingulate circuitry in human maternal behavior. Biol Psychiatry 51(6):431–445.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01284-7 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. MacNeilage PF, Rogers LJ, Vallortigara G (2009) Origins of the left and right brain. Sci Am 301(1):60–67.  https://doi.org/10.1038/scientificamerican0709-60 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Manning JT, Heaton R, Chamberlain AT (1994) Left-side cradling: similarities and differences between apes and humans. J Hum Evol 26(1):77–83.  https://doi.org/10.1006/jhev.1994.1005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Minagawa-Kawai Y, Matsuoka S, Dan I, Naoi N, Nakamura K, Kojima S (2009) Prefrontal activation associated with social attachment: facial-emotion recognition in mothers and infants. Cereb Cortex 19(2):284–292.  https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhn081 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Nagasawa M, Kawai E, Mogi K, Kikusui T (2013) Dogs show left facial lateralization upon reunion with their owners. Behav Process 98:112–116.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2013.05.012 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Nagy M, Akos Z, Biro D, Vicsek T (2010) Hierarchical group dynamics in pigeon flocks. Nature 464(7290):890–893.  https://doi.org/10.1038/nature08891 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Pfannkuche KA, Bouma A, Groothuis TG (2009) Does testosterone affect lateralization of brain and behaviour? A meta-analysis in humans and other animal species. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 364(1519):929–942.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0282 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Quaresmini C, Forrester GS, Spiezio C, Vallortigara G (2014) Social environment elicits lateralized behaviors in gorillas and chimpanzees. J Comp Psychol 128(3):276–284.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036355 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Reber J, Tranel D (2017) Sex differences in the functional lateralization of emotion and decision making in the human brain. J Neurosci Res 95(1-2):270–278.  https://doi.org/10.1002/jnr.23829 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  47. Rogers LJ (2010) Relevance of brain and behavioural lateralization to animal welfare. Appl Anim Behav Sci 127 (1–2):1–11.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2010.06.008
  48. Rogers LJ (2017) Eye and ear preferences. In: Rogers LJ, Vallortigara G (eds) Lateralized brain functions: methods in human and non-human species. Springer, New York, pp 79–102.  https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4939-6725-4_3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Rogers LJ, Vallortigara G, Andrew RJ (2013) Divided brains: the biology and behavior of brain asymmetries. Cambridge University Press, New York.  https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511793899 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Rosa Salva O, Regolin L, Mascalzoni E, Vallortigara G (2012) Cerebral and behavioural asymmetries in animal social recognition. Comp Cogn Behav Rev 7:110–138.  https://doi.org/10.3819/ccbr.2012.70006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Sakai M, Hishii T, Takeda S, Kohshima S (2006) Laterality of flipper rubbing behaviour in wild bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops aduncus): caused by asymmetry of eye use? Behav Brain Res 170(2):204–210.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2006.02.018 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Scheumann M, Joly-Radko M, Leliveld L, Zimmermann E (2011) Does body posture influence hand preference in an ancestral primate model? BMC Evol Biol 11(1):52.  https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2148-11-52 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  53. Scheumann M, Zimmermann E (2008) Sex-specific asymmetries in communication sound perception are not related to hand preference in an early primate. BMC Biol 6(1):3.  https://doi.org/10.1186/1741-7007-6-3 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  54. Scola C, Arciszewski T, Measelle J, Vauclair J (2013) Infant-holding bias variations in mother–child relationships: a longitudinal study. Eur J Dev Psychol 10(6):707–722.  https://doi.org/10.1080/17405629.2013.791230 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Seligmann H (2002) Behavioural and morphological asymmetries in hindlimbs of Hoplodactylus duvaucelii (Lacertilia: Gekkonomorpha: Gekkota: Diplodactylinae). Laterality 7(3):277–283.  https://doi.org/10.1080/13576500244000067 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Siniscalchi M, Dimatteo S, Pepe AM, Sasso R, Quaranta A (2012) Visual lateralization in wild striped dolphins (Stenella coeruleoalba) in response to stimuli with different degrees of familiarity. PLoS One 7(1):e30001.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0030001 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  57. Sovrano VA, Rainoldi C, Bisazza A, Vallortigara G (1999) Roots of brain specializations: preferential left-eye use during mirror-image inspection in six species of teleost fish. Behav Brain Res 106(1-2):175–180.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4328(99)00105-9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Suedmeyer WK (2006) Special senses. In: Fowler ME, Mikota SK (eds) Elephant biology, medicine, and surgery. Blackwell Publishing, Ames.  https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470344484.ch31 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Sukumar R (2003) The living elephants: evolutionary ecology, behaviour, and conservation. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  60. Todd BK, Banerjee R (2016) Lateralization of infant holding by mothers: a longitudinal evaluation of variations over the first 12 weeks. Laterality 21(1):12–33.  https://doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2015.1059434 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  61. Vallortigara G, Rogers LJ (2005) Survival with an asymmetrical brain: advantages and disadvantages of cerebral lateralization. Behav Brain Sci 28:575–589PubMedGoogle Scholar
  62. Vallortigara G, Rogers LJ, Bisazza A (1999) Possible evolutionary origins of cognitive brain lateralization. Brain Res Rev 30(2):164–175.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0165-0173(99)00012-0 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  63. Varma S, Baskaran N, Sukumar R (2012) Field key for elephant population estimation and age and sex classification. Asian Nature Conservation Foundation, Innovation Centre, Indian Institute of Science, BangaloreGoogle Scholar
  64. Versace E, Morgante M, Pulina G, Vallortigara G (2007) Behavioural lateralization in sheep (Ovis aries). Behav Brain Res 184(1):72–80.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2007.06.016 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  65. Vidya TNC, Prasad D, Ghosh A (2014) Individual identification in Asian elephants. Gajah 40:3–17Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Vertebrate Zoology, Faculty of BiologySaint Petersburg State UniversitySaint PetersburgRussia
  2. 2.Elephant, Forest and Environment Conservation TrustColombo 5Sri Lanka
  3. 3.Smithsonian Conservation Biology InstituteFront RoyalUSA
  4. 4.Laboratory of Molecular Neurobiology, Department of Ecological PhysiologyInstitute of Experimental MedicineSt. PetersburgRussia

Personalised recommendations