Advertisement

Male white-handed gibbons flexibly time duet contributions

  • Thomas A. TerlephEmail author
  • S. Malaivijitnond
  • U. H. Reichard
Original Article

Abstract

Vocal duetting occurs when two individuals produce repeated, stereotyped vocalizations, often with alternating contributions. It evolved independently in many pair living taxa. Among hominoids, only hylobatids duet, but little is known about how mated pairs coordinate singing and if individuals adjust their song to spectral and temporal aspects of another’s song. If they do, this would demonstrate vocal flexibility not yet well documented in apes. To test this hypothesis, we analyzed duets of wild white-handed gibbons (Hylobates lar), quantifying female notes and the timing of male notes relative to them. We measured changes in female notes that preceded her great call phrase and that she initiated only after her mate had stopped singing. We predicted males would suspend their own song during a female’s great call phrase in anticipation of her climax, and thus should interrupt abnormal female calls, because these typically fail to climax. We compared (1) interrupted great call phrases females aborted, (2) interrupted phrases that were completed, and (3) uninterrupted, completed phrases. We found that abnormal great call phrases were interrupted by males, likely because the male anticipated that the phrase would be abandoned before reaching its climax. Although female call phrases varied in length, we also found that males replied in close synchrony with their ending. Subtle spectral and temporal variations in song influenced the timing of a mate’s singing and thus the structure and delivery of duets. Although ape vocal behavior is thought to be largely innate, our findings show unexpected flexibility in song expression.

Significance statement

Vocal duets by mated pairs occur in many animal species but only in one ape family, the hylobatids (gibbons and siamang). Although gibbon duetting has been described for many species, little is still known about how partners coordinate singing. We show that individuals rapidly and flexibly adjust their song in direct response to changes in a mate’s singing. This coordinated adjustment to a mate’s ongoing singing likely allows for precise turn-taking during duets, which centers around the female great call phrase, and especially its climax notes. Although vocalizations of nonhuman apes are thought to be largely developmentally fixed, this plasticity suggests that turn-taking duets may be an exception. Turn-taking duetting requires gibbons to adjust their singing to that of the partner, perhaps similar to vocal exchange behaviors that contributed to increased flexibility in the precursors of speech.

Keywords

Duetting Gibbons Song Ape communication Turn-taking Vocal plasticity 

Notes

Acknowledgments

Thanks to the Nacey Maggioncalda Foundation for a Goldberg Research Grant to Thomas Terleph and to the National Research Council of Thailand for research permits and the Department of the National Parks, Wildlife and Plant Conservation for park permits. We also thank Dr. David Watts and two anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments.

Funding

This study was funded by a Goldberg Research Grant, Nacey Maggioncalda Foundation to TAT.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

Research permits were issued by the National Research Council of Thailand (NRCT) and the Department of National Parks, Wildlife and Plant Conservation (DNP) of Thailand, and the research protocol was approved by the Institutional Animal Care Committees of Sacred Heart University and Southern Illinois University Carbondale, which adhere to the American Society of Primatologists Principles for the Ethical Treatment of Non-Human Primates. All applicable international, national, and/or institutional guidelines for the care and use of animals were followed.

References

  1. Bailey WJ (2003) Insect duets: underlying mechanisms and their evolution. Physiol Entomol 28(3):157–174.  https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-3032.2003.00337.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Benedict L (2008) Unusually high levels of extrapair paternity in a duetting songbird with long-term pair bonds. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 62(6):983–988.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-007-0524-x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y (1995) Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J R Stat Soc B 57:289–300Google Scholar
  4. Biben M, Symmes D, Masataka N (1986) Temporal and structural analysis of affiliative vocal exchanges in squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus). Behaviour 98(1):259–273.  https://doi.org/10.1163/156853986X00991 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Brockelman WY, Schilling D (1984) Inheritance of stereotyped gibbon calls. Nature 312(5995):634–636.  https://doi.org/10.1038/312634a0 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Brockelman WY, Srikosamatara S (1984) Maintenance and evolution of social structure in gibbons. In: Preuschoft H, Chivers DJ, Brockelman WY, Creel N (eds) The lesser apes: evolutionary and behavioural biology. Edinburgh University Press, Edinburgh, pp 298–323Google Scholar
  7. Cheyne SM (2010) Behavioural ecology of gibbons (Hylobates albibarbis) in a degraded peat swamp forest. In: Rowe N (ed) All the world’s primates. Pogonias press, Charlestown, pp 121–152Google Scholar
  8. Chivers DJ (1976) Communication within and between family groups of siamang (Symphalangus syndactylus). Behaviour 57(1):116–135.  https://doi.org/10.1163/156853976X00136 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Chivers DJ (1977) The ecology of gibbons: some preliminary considerations based on observations in the Malay Peninsula. In: Prasad MRN, Anad Kumar TC (eds) Use of non-human primates in biomedical research. Indian National Science Academy, New Delhi, pp 85–105Google Scholar
  10. Chivers DJ (1989) The social behavior of the lesser apes. In: Seth PK, Seth S (eds) Perspectives in primate biology, vol 3. Today & Tomorrow’s Printers and Publishers, New Delhi, pp 141–155Google Scholar
  11. Chow CP, Mitchell JF, Miller CT (2015) Vocal turn-taking in a non-human primate is learned during ontogeny. Proc R Soc B 282(1807):20150069.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2015.0069 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Clarke E, Reichard UH, Zuberbühler K (2006) The syntax and meaning of wild gibbon songs. PLoS One 1(1):e73.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0000073 PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  13. Cowlishaw G (1992) Song function in gibbons. Behaviour 121(1):131–153.  https://doi.org/10.1163/156853992X00471 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Crockford C, Herbinger I, Vigilant L, Boesch C (2004) Wild chimpanzees produce group-specific calls: a case for vocal learning? Ethology 110(3):221–243.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2004.00968.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Dallmann R, Geissmann T (2001a) Different levels of variability in the female song of wild silvery gibbons (Hylobates moloch). Behaviour 138(5):629–648.  https://doi.org/10.1163/156853901316924511 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Dallmann R, Geissmann T (2001b) Individuality in the female songs of wild silvery gibbons (Hylobates moloch) on Java, Indonesia. Contrib Zool 70:41–50Google Scholar
  17. Dallmann R, Geissmann T (2009) Individual and geographical variability in the songs of wild silvery gibbons (Hylobates moloch) on Java, Indonesia. In: Lappan S, Whittacker DJ (eds) The gibbons: new perspectives on small ape socioecology and population biology. Springer, New York, pp 91–110.  https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-88604-6_6 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Egnor SE, Hauser MD (2004) A paradox in the evolution of primate vocal learning. Trends Neurosci 27(11):649–654.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2004.08.009 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Farabaugh SM (1982) The ecological and social significance of duetting. In: Kroodsma DE, Miller EH (eds) Acoustic communication in birds, vol 2. Academic Press, New York, pp 85–124Google Scholar
  20. Fedurek P, Schel AM, Slocombe KE (2013) The acoustic structure of chimpanzee pant-hooting facilitates chorusing. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 67:1781–1789CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Fitch WT (2006) Production of vocalizations in mammals. In: Brown K (ed) Encyclopedia of language and linguistics. Elsevier, Oxford, pp 115–121.  https://doi.org/10.1016/B0-08-044854-2/00821-X CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Geissmann T (1984) Inheritance of song parameters in the gibbon song, analysed in 2 hybrid gibbons (Hylobates pileatus × H. lar). Folia Primatol 42(3-4):216–235.  https://doi.org/10.1159/000156165 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. GeissmannT (1993) Evolution of communication in gibbons (Hylobatidae). Dissertation, Zürich UniversityGoogle Scholar
  24. Geissmann T (1999) Duet songs of the siamang, Hylobates syndactylus: II. Testing the pair-bonding hypothesis during a partner exchange. Behaviour 136(8):1005–1039.  https://doi.org/10.1163/156853999501694 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Geissmann T (2000) Duet songs of the siamang, Hylobates syndactylus: I. Structure and organisation. Primate Rep 56:33–60Google Scholar
  26. Geissmann T (2002) Duet-splitting and the evolution of gibbon songs. Biol Rev 77(1):57–76.  https://doi.org/10.1017/S1464793101005826 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Geissmann T, Orgeldinger M (2000) The relationship between duet songs and pair bonds in siamangs, Hylobates syndactylus. Anim Behav 60(6):805–809.  https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2000.1540 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Hafen T, Neveu H, Rumpler Y, Wilden I, Zimmermann E (1998) Acoustically dimorphic advertisement calls separate morphologically and genetically homogenous populations of the grey mouse lemur (Microcebus murinus). Folia Primatol 69(Suppl 1):342–356.  https://doi.org/10.1159/000052723 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Haimoff EH (1984) Acoustic and organizational features of gibbon songs. In: Preuschoft H, Chivers DJ, Brockelman WY, Creel N (eds) The lesser apes: evolutionary and behavioural biology. Edinburgh University Press, Edinburgh, pp 333–353Google Scholar
  30. Haimoff EH (1986) Convergence in the duetting of monogamous old world primates. J Hum Evol 15(1):51–59.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0047-2484(86)80065-3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Haimoff EH, Gittins SP (1985) Individuality in the songs of wild agile gibbons (Hylobates agilis) of Peninsular Malaysia. Am J Primatol 8(3):239–247.  https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.1350080306 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Haimoff EH, Tilson RL (1985) Individuality in the female songs of wild Kloss’ gibbons (Hylobates klossii) on Siberut Island, Indonesia. Folia Primatol 4:129–137CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Hall ML (2004) A review of hypotheses for the functions of avian duetting. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 55(5):415–430.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-003-0741-x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Hammerschmidt K, Fischer J (2008) Constraints in primate vocal production. In: Griebel U, Oller K (eds) The evolution of communicative creativity: from fixed signals to contextual flexibility. The MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 93–119.  https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9780262151214.003.0005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Haraway MM, Maples EG (1998) Flexibility in the species-typical songs of gibbons. Primates 39(1):1–12.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02557739 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Henry L, Craig AJFK, Lemasson A, Hausberger M (2016) Social coordination in animal vocal interactions. Is there any evidence of turn-taking? The starling as an animal model. In: Holler J, Kendrick KH, Casillas M, Levinson SC (eds) Turn-taking in human communicative interaction. Frontiers Media SA, Lausanne, pp 39–59Google Scholar
  37. Hoit JD (1995) Influence of body position on breathing and its implications for the evaluation and treatment of speech and voice disorders. J Voice 9(4):341–347.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0892-1997(05)80196-1 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Hopkins WD, Taglialatela J, Leavens DA (2007) Chimpanzees differentially produce novel vocalizations to capture the attention of a human. Anim Behav 73(2):281–286.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.08.004 PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  39. Jarvis ED (2007) Neural systems for vocal learning in birds and humans: a synopsis. J Ornithol 148(S1):35–44.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10336-007-0243-0 PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  40. Jürgens U (1995) Neuronal control of vocal production in nonhuman and human primates. In: Zimmerman E, Newman JD, Jürgens U (eds) Current topics in primate vocal communication. Plenum, New York, pp 199–202.  https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4757-9930-9_10 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Keith SA, Waller MS, Geissmann T (2009) Vocal diversity of Kloss’s gibbons (Hylobates klossii) in the Mentawai Islands, Indonesia. In: Lappan S, Whittacker DJ (eds) The gibbons: new perspectives on small ape socioecology and population biology. Springer, New York, pp 51–71.  https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-88604-6_4 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Koda H (2004) Flexibility and context-sensitivity during the vocal exchange of coo calls in wild Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata yakui). Behaviour 141(10):1279–1296.  https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539042729685 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Koda H, Lemasson A, Oyakawa C, Rizaldi PJ, Masataka N (2013) Possible role of mother-daughter vocal interactions on the development of species-specific song in gibbons. PLoS One 8(8):e71432.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0071432 PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  44. Langmore NE (2002) Vocal duetting: definitions, discoveries and directions. Trends Ecol Evol 17(10):451–452.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0169-5347(02)02611-3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Leighton DR (1987) Gibbons: territoriality and monogamy. In: Smuts BB, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Wrangham RW, Struhsaker TT (eds) Primate societies. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 135–145Google Scholar
  46. Lemasson A, Hausberger M (2004) Patterns of vocal sharing and social dynamics in a captive group of Campbell’s monkeys. J Comp Psychol 118(3):347–359.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.118.3.347 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Lemasson A, Gandon E, Hausberger M (2010) Attention to elders’ voice in nonhuman primates. Biol Lett 6(3):325–328.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2009.0875 CrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  48. Lemasson A, Ouattara K, Petit EJ, Zuberbühler K (2011) Social learning of vocal structure in a nonhuman primate? BMC Evol Biol 11(1):362.  https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2148-11-362 PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  49. Levinson SC (2016) Turn-taking in human communication—origins and implications for language processing. Trends Cogn Sci 20(1):6–14.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.10.010 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Maples EG, Haraway MM, Hutto CW (1989) Development of coordinated singing in a newly formed siamang pair. Zoo Biol 8(4):367–378.  https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.1430080407 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Marshall JT, Marshall ER (1976) Gibbons and their territorial songs. Science 193(4249):235–237.  https://doi.org/10.1126/science.193.4249.235 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Méndez-Cárdenas MG, Zimmermann E (2009) Duetting—a mechanism to strengthen pair bonds in a dispersed pair-living primate (Lepilemur edwardsi)? Am J Phys Anthropol 139(4):523–532.  https://doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.21017 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Miller CT, Wang X (2006) Sensory-motor interactions modulate a primate vocal behavior: antiphonal calling in common marmosets. J Comp Physiol A 192(1):27–38.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00359-005-0043-z CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Mitani JC (1985) Gibbon song duets and intergroup spacing. Behaviour 92(1):59–96.  https://doi.org/10.1163/156853985X00389 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Müller AE, Anzenberger G (2002) Duetting in the titi monkey Callicebus cupreus: structure, pair specificity and development of duets. Folia Primatol 73(2-3):104–115.  https://doi.org/10.1159/000064788 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Nietsch A (1999) Duet vocalizations among different populations of Sulawesi tarsiers. Int J Primatol 20(4):567–583.  https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1020342807709 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Ouattara K, Lemmason A, Zuberbühler K (2009) Campbell’s monkeys concatenate vocalizations into context-specific call sequences. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106(51):22026–22031.  https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0908118106 PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  58. Owren MJ, Dieter JA, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (1993) Vocalizations of rhesus (Macaca mulatta) and Japanese (M. fuscata) macaques cross-fostered between species show evidence of only limited modification. Dev Psychobiol 26(7):389–406.  https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.420260703 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Oyakawa C, Koda H, Sugiura H (2007) Acoustic features contributing to the individuality of wild agile gibbon (Hylobates agilis agilis) songs. Am J Primatol 69(7):777–790.  https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20390 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Palombit RA (1994) Dynamic pair bonds in hylobatids: implications regarding monogamous social systems. Behaviour 128(1):65–101.  https://doi.org/10.1163/156853994X00055 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Raemaekers JJ, Raemaekers PM (1985) Field playback of loud calls to gibbons (Hylobates lar): territorial, sex-specific and species-specific responses. Anim Behav 33(2):481–493.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(85)80071-3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Raemaekers JJ, Raemaekers PM, Haimoff EH (1984) Loud calls of the gibbon (Hylobates lar): repertoire, organization and context. Behaviour 91(1):146–189.  https://doi.org/10.1163/156853984X00263 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Reichard UH (2009) Social organization and mating system of Khao Yai white-handed gibbons, 1992–2006. In: Lappan S, Whittacker DJ (eds) The gibbons: new perspectives on small ape socioecology and population biology. Springer, New York, pp 347–384.  https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-88604-6_17 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Reichard UH, Ganpanakngan M, Barelli C (2012) White handed gibbons of Khao Yai: social flexibility, reproductive strategies, and a slow life history. In: Kappeler PM, Watts D (eds) Long-term field studies of primates. Springer, Berlin, pp 237–258.  https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-22514-7_11 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Reichard UH, Barelli C, Hirai H, Nowak MG (2016) The evolution of gibbons and siamang. In: Reichard UH (ed) Evolution of gibbons and siamang, phylogeny, morphology, and cognition. Springer, New York, pp 3–41.  https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4939-5614-2_1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Roy S, Miller CT, Gottsch D, Wang X (2011) Vocal control by the common marmoset in the presence of interfering noise. J Exp Biol 214(21):3619–3629.  https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.056101 PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  67. Schulz TM, Whitehead H, Gero S, Rendell L (2008) Overlapping and matching of codas in vocal interactions between sperm whales: insights into communication function. Anim Behav 76(6):1977–1988.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.07.032 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (1980) The ontogeny of vervet monkey alarm-calling behavior: a preliminary report. Z Tierpsychol 54:37–56CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Snowdon CT, Elowson AM (1999) Pygmy marmosets modify call structure when paired. Ethology 105(10):893–908.  https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1439-0310.1999.00483.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Stivers T, Enfield NJ, Brown P et al (2009) Universals and cultural variation in turn-taking in conversation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106:587–592CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Sugiura H (1993) Temporal and acoustic correlates in vocal exchange of coo calls in Japanese macaques. Behaviour 124(3):207–225.  https://doi.org/10.1163/156853993X00588 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Takahashi D, Narayanan D, Ghazanfar AA (2013) Coupled oscillator dynamics of vocal turn-taking in monkeys. Curr Biol 23(21):2162–2168.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.09.005 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Tanaka T, Sugiura H, Masataka N (2006) Cross-sectional and longitudinal studies of the development of group differences in acoustic features of coo calls in two groups of Japanese macaques. Ethology 112(1):7–21.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2006.01103.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Tenaza RR (1975) Territory and monogamy among Kloss’ gibbons (Hylobates klossii) in Siberut Island, Indonesia. Folia Primatol 24(1):60–80.  https://doi.org/10.1159/000155685 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Terleph TA, Malaivijitnond S, Reichard UH (2015) The lar gibbon (Hylobates lar) great call reveals individual caller identity. Am J Primatol 77(7):811–821.  https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.22406 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Terleph TA, Malaivijitnond S, Reichard UH (2016) Age related decline in female lar gibbon great call performance suggests that call features correlate with physical condition. BMC Evol Biol 16(1):4.  https://doi.org/10.1186/s12862-015-0578-8 PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  77. Thorpe WH (1972) Duetting and antiphonal singing in birds: its extent and significance. Behaviour Suppl 18:1–197Google Scholar
  78. Tobias ML, Viswanathan SS, Kelley DB (1998) Rapping a female receptive call, initiates male–female duets in the South African clawed frog. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 95(4):1870–1875.  https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.95.4.1870 PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  79. von Helversen D (1980) Structure and function of antiphonal duets. Proc Int Ornithol Congr Berl 17:682–688Google Scholar
  80. von Helversen D, Balakrishnan R, von Helversen O (2004) Acoustic communication in a duetting grasshopper: receiver response variability, male strategies and signal design. Anim Behav 68(1):131–144.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.10.020 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Wilson M, Wilson TP (2005) An oscillator model of the timing of turn-taking. Psychon Bull Rev 12(6):957–968.  https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03206432 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of BiologySacred Heart UniversityFairfieldUSA
  2. 2.National Primate Research Center of ThailandCham Phak PheoThailand
  3. 3.Department of Biology, Faculty of ScienceChulalongkorn UniversityBangkokThailand
  4. 4.Department of Anthropology and Center for EcologySouthern Illinois University CarbondaleCarbondaleUSA

Personalised recommendations