Advertisement

Ontogenetic change in determinants of social network position in the spotted hyena

  • Julie W. Turner
  • Patrick S. Bills
  • Kay E. Holekamp
Featured Student Research Paper

Abstract

Social development is crucial in the ontogeny of animals living in complex societies and has lasting consequences in adulthood. Spotted hyenas (Crocuta crocuta) live in fission-fusion societies as complex as those of cercopithecine primates. The social positions adult hyenas hold within their groups are complex and varied, but little is known about how those positons emerge and change over the course of development. Using social network analysis (SNA), we tested predictions of hypotheses suggesting that sex and dominance status affect the social network positions of young hyenas across three stages of ontogeny: (1) while living at the communal den, (2) den independent but pre-reproductive, and (3) early adulthood. By examining rates at which hyenas were found alone and their association networks, aggression networks, and affiliation networks, we observed striking changes in individuals’ network positions across ontogeny, as well as pronounced sex differences. With the exception of rates at which individuals were found alone, which increased over ontogeny, most social network position metrics decreased greatly from infancy to adulthood. However, females showed considerably more rank-related variation in this trajectory than did males. Overall, social rank had stronger effects on the development of social network positions in females than males. Thus, females and males have different social development trajectories that appear to prepare them for their different respective futures of integrating into their natal clan or dispersing to a new one.

Significance statement

Social development is difficult to study, particularly in long-lived gregarious mammals, so little is known about the variables shaping the emergence during ontogeny of social roles played by adults. We used social network analysis to investigate how sex and rank affect the social positions of maturing spotted hyenas as their positions change across ontogenetic development. Females develop more complex social positions than males early in life, with strategies that vary with their dominance status. Although males are just as well connected to group-mates as females when they are cubs, they generally disengage from the natal clan as they mature; males clearly do not disperse because they are aggressively expelled from their natal group. Our data suggest for the first time that social development appears to prepare females and males for their alternative futures of philopatry or dispersal.

Keywords

Spotted hyena Social network Social position Social development Ontogeny Social bonds 

Notes

Acknowledgments

We thank the Kenyan National Commission for Science, Technology, and Innovation for permission to conduct this research and the Kenya Wildlife Service, Narok County Government and the Senior Warden of the Masai Mara National Reserve for assistance. We are indebted to all those who have contributed to long-term data collection on the Mara Hyena Project. Special thanks to T. Getty, L. Smale, J. Neal, and A. Wolfe for helpful comments along the way. We would also like to thank three very thoughtful anonymous reviewers for their comments. This research was supported by National Science Foundation Grants DEB1353110 and OISE1556407 to KEH, a Student Research Grant from the Animal Behavior Society to JWT, a Grant-in-Aid from the American Society of Mammalogists to JWT, and a Dissertation Continuation Fellowship to JWT from the College of Natural Science at Michigan State University.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving animals were in accordance with the ethical standards of Michigan State University and following all applicable guidelines in Kenya. Ethical approval for use of animals in this study was issued by Michigan State University under IACUC approval no. 05/11-110-00 on 22 August 2013.

Supplementary material

265_2017_2426_MOESM1_ESM.docx (618 kb)
ESM 1 (DOCX 617 kb)

References

  1. Altmann J (1974) Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. Behaviour 49(3):227–267.  https://doi.org/10.1163/156853974X00534 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Anderson CO, Mason WA (1978) Competitive social strategies in groups of deprived and experienced rhesus monkeys. Dev Psychobiol 11(4):289–299.  https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.420110402 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Aplin LM, Farine DR, Morand-Ferron J, Cole EF, Cockburn A, Sheldon BC (2013) Individual personalities predict social behaviour in wild networks of great tits (Parus major). Ecol Lett 16(11):1365–1372.  https://doi.org/10.1111/ele.12181 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Archie EA, Chiyo PI (2012) Elephant behaviour and conservation: social relationships, the effects of poaching, and genetic tools for management. Mol Ecol 21(3):765–778.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-294X.2011.05237.x PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Archie EA, Tung J, Clark M, Altmann J, Alberts SC (2014) Social affiliation matters: both same-sex and opposite-sex relationships predict survival in wild female baboons. Proc R Soc B 281(1793):20141261.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2014.1261 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Barabasi A, Albert R (1999) Emergence of scaling in random networks. Science 286(5439):509–512PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Barocas A, Ilany A, Koren L, Kam M, Geffen E (2011) Variance in centrality within rock hyrax social networks predicts adult longevity. PLoS One 6(7):e22375.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0022375 PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Belsky J, Steinberg L, Draper P (1991) Childhood experience, interpersonal development, and reproductive strategy: an evolutionary theory of socialization. Child Dev 62(4):647–670.  https://doi.org/10.2307/1131166 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Belsky J, Vandell DL, Burchinal M, Clarke-Stewart KA, McCartney K, Owen MT, The NICHD Early Child Care Research Network (2007) Are there long-term effects of early child care? Child Dev 78(2):681–701.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01021.x PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Bercovitch FB (1991) Social stratification, social strategies, and reproductive success in primates. Ethol Sociobiol 12(4):315–333.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0162-3095(91)90023-J CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Berman CM (1982) The ontogeny of social relationships with group companions among free-ranging infant rhesus monkeys. II. Differentiation and attractiveness. Anim Behav 30(1):149–162.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(82)80250-9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Boydston EE, Morelli TL, Holekamp KE (2001) Sex differences in territorial behavior exhibited by the spotted hyena (Hyaenidae, Crocuta crocuta). Ethology 107(5):369–385.  https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1439-0310.2001.00672.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Boydston EE, Kapheim KM, Van Horn RC, Smale L, Holekamp KE (2005) Sexually dimorphic patterns of space use throughout ontogeny in the spotted hyena (Crocuta crocuta). J Zool 267(03):271–281.  https://doi.org/10.1017/S0952836905007478 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Brent LJN, Ruiz-Lambides A, Platt ML (2017) Family network size and survival across the lifespan of female macaques. Proc R Soc B 284(1854):20170515.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2017.0515 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Butts CT (2008) Network: a package for managing relational data in R. J Stat Softw 24:1–36PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  16. Butts CT (2015) Network: classes for relational data. https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/network/vignettes/networkVignette.pdf
  17. Cairns SJ, Schwager SJ (1987) A comparison of association indices. Anim Behav 35(5):1454–1469.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(87)80018-0 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Campbell CJ, Fuentes A, MacKinnon K, Bearder SK, Stumpf RM (2011) Primates in perspective. Oxford University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  19. Chiyo PI, Moss CJ, Alberts SC (2012) The influence of life history milestones and association networks on crop-raiding behavior in male African elephants. PLoS One 7(2):e31382.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0031382 PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Crockford C, Wittig RM, Whitten PL, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (2008) Social stressors and coping mechanisms in wild female baboons (Papio hamadryas ursinus). Horm Behav 53(1):254–265.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2007.10.007 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Croft DP, James R, Krause J (2008) Exploring animal social networks. Princeton University Press, Princeton.  https://doi.org/10.1515/9781400837762 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Croft DP, Krause J, Darden SK (2009) Behavioural trait assortment in a social network: patterns and implications. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 63(10):1495–1503.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-009-0802-x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. East ML, Hofer H (2001) Male spotted hyenas (Crocuta crocuta) queue for status in social groups dominated by females. Behav Ecol 12(5):558–568.  https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/12.5.558 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Ellis L (1995) Dominance and reproductive success among nonhuman animals: a cross-species comparison. Ethol Sociobiol 16(4):257–333.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0162-3095(95)00050-U CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Evans KE, Harris S (2008) Adolescence in male African elephants, Loxodonta africana, and the importance of sociality. Anim Behav 76(3):779–787.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.03.019 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Fournier DA, Skaug HJ, Ancheta J, Ianelli J, Magnusson A, Maunder MN, Nielsen A, Sibert J (2012) AD Model Builder: using automatic differentiation for statistical inference of highly parameterized complex nonlinear models. Optim Method Softw 27(2):233–249.  https://doi.org/10.1080/10556788.2011.597854 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Frank LG (1986) Social organization of the spotted hyaena (Crocuta crocuta). I. Demography. Anim Behav 34(5):1500–1509.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(86)80220-2 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Frank LG, Glickman SE, Licht P (1991) Fatal sibling aggression, precocial development, and androgens in neonatal spotted hyenas. Science 252(5006):702–704.  https://doi.org/10.1126/science.2024122 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Granovetter MS (1973) The strength of weak ties. Am J Sociol 78(6):1360–1380.  https://doi.org/10.1086/225469 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Hamede RK, Bashford J, McCallum H, Jones M (2009) Contact networks in a wild Tasmanian devil (Sarcophilus harrisii) population: using social network analysis to reveal seasonal variability in social behaviour and its implications for transmission of devil facial tumour disease. Ecol Lett 12(11):1147–1157.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1461-0248.2009.01370.x PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Handcock MS, Hunter DR, Butts CT, Goodreau SM, Morris M (2008) Statnet: software tools for the representation, visualization, analysis and simulation of network data. J Stat Softw 24(1):1548–7660CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Hanneman RA, Riddle M (2005) Introduction to social network methods. University of California, RiversideGoogle Scholar
  33. Hinde RA (1976) Interactions, relationships and social structure. Man 11(1):1–17.  https://doi.org/10.2307/2800384 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Hinde RA, Atkinson S (1970) Assessing the roles of social partners in maintaining mutual proximity, as exemplified by mother-infant relations in rhesus monkeys. Anim Behav 18:169–176.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0003-3472(70)90087-4 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Hobson EA, DeDeo S (2015) Social feedback and the emergence of rank in animal society. PLoS Comput Biol 11(9):e1004411.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004411 PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Hobson EA, Avery ML, Wright TF (2013) An analytical framework for quantifying and testing patterns of temporal dynamics in social networks. Anim Behav 85(1):83–96.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.10.010 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Hock K, Ng KL, Fefferman NH (2010) Systems approach to studying animal sociality: individual position versus group organization in dynamic social network models. PLoS One 5(12):e15789.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0015789 PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Hofer H, East ML (2003) Behavioral processes and costs of co-existence in female spotted hyenas: a life history perspective. Evol Ecol 17(4):315–331.  https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1027352517231 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Holekamp KE, Smale L (1991) Dominance acquisition during mammalian social development: the “inheritance” of maternal rank. Am Zool 31(2):306–317.  https://doi.org/10.1093/icb/31.2.306 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Holekamp KE, Smale L (1993) Ontogeny of dominance in free-living spotted hyaenas: juvenile rank relations with other immature individuals. Anim Behav 46(3):451–466.  https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1993.1214 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Holekamp KE, Smale L (1998a) Behavioral development in the spotted hyena. Bioscience 48(12):997–1005.  https://doi.org/10.2307/1313456 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Holekamp KE, Smale L (1998b) Dispersal status influences hormones and behavior in the male spotted hyena. Horm Behav 33(3):205–216.  https://doi.org/10.1006/hbeh.1998.1450 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Holekamp KE, Smale L, Szykman M (1996) Rank and reproduction in the female spotted hyaena. J Reprod Fertil 108(2):229–237.  https://doi.org/10.1530/jrf.0.1080229 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Holekamp KE, Cooper SM, Katona CI, Berry NA, Frank LG, Smale L (1997) Patterns of association among female spotted hyenas (Crocuta crocuta). J Mammal 78(1):55–64.  https://doi.org/10.2307/1382638 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Holekamp KE, Smith JE, Strelioff CC, Van Horn RC, Watts HE (2012) Society, demography and genetic structure in the spotted hyena. Mol Ecol 21(3):613–632.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-294X.2011.05240.x PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Holekamp KE, Swanson EM, Van Meter PE (2013) Developmental constraints on behavioural flexibility. Philos T Roy Soc B 368(1618):20120350.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2012.0350 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Holekamp KE, Dantzer B, Stricker G, Yoshida KCS, Benson-Amram S (2015) Brains, brawn and sociality: a hyaena’s tale. Anim Behav 103:237–248.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.01.023 PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Höner OP, Wachter B, Hofer H, Wilhelm K, Thierer D, Trillmich F, Burke T, East ML (2010) The fitness of dispersing spotted hyaena sons is influenced by maternal social status. Nat Commun 1:60–67Google Scholar
  49. Joffe TH (1997) Social pressures have selected for an extended juvenile period in primates. J Hum Evol 32(6):593–605.  https://doi.org/10.1006/jhev.1997.0140 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Kruuk H (1972) The spotted hyena: a study of predation and social behavior. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  51. Kurvers RHJM, Adamczyk VMAP, van Wieren SE, Prins HHT (2011) The effect of boldness on decision-making in barnacle geese is group-size-dependent. Proc R Soc Lond B 278(1714):2018–2024.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2010.2266 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Lehmann J, Ross C (2011) Baboon (Papio anubis) social complexity—a network approach. Am J Primatol 73(8):775–789.  https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20967 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Makagon MM, McCowan B, Mench JA (2012) How can social network analysis contribute to social behavior research in applied ethology? Appl Anim Behav Sci 138(3-4):152–161.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2012.02.003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Mason WA (1968) Early social deprivation in nonhuman primates: implications for human behavior. In: Glass DS (ed) Environmental influences. Rockefeller University Press, New York, pp 70–101Google Scholar
  55. Massen JJM, Koski SE (2014) Chimps of a feather sit together: chimpanzee friendships are based on homophily in personality. Evol Hum Behav 35(1):1–8.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2013.08.008 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. McCowan B, Beisner BA, Capitanio JP, Jackson ME, Cameron AN, Seil S, Atwill ER, Hsieh F (2011) Network stability is a balancing act of personality, power, and conflict dynamics in rhesus macaque societies. PLoS One 6(8):e22350.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0022350 PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. McDonald DB (2007) Predicting fate from early connectivity in a social network. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104(26):10910–10914.  https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0701159104 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Belsky J, Silva PA (1992) Childhood experience and the onset of menarche: a test of a sociobiological model. Child Dev 63(1):47–58.  https://doi.org/10.2307/1130900 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Newman MEJ (2003) The structure and function of complex networks. SIAM Rev 45(2):167–256.  https://doi.org/10.1137/S003614450342480 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Onyango PO, Gesquiere LR, Altmann J, Alberts SC (2012) Testosterone positively associated with both male mating effort and paternal behavior in savanna baboons (Papio cynocephalus). Horm Behav 63:430–436PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Onyango PO, Gesquiere LR, Altmann J, Alberts SC (2013) Puberty and dispersal in a wild primate population. Horm Behav 64(2):240–249.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2013.02.014 PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Pereira ME (1988) Effects of age and sex on intra-group spacing behaviour in juvenile savannah baboons, Papio cynocephalus cynocephalus. Anim Behav 36(1):184–204.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(88)80262-8 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Pinter-Wollman N, Hobson EA, Smith JE, Edelman AJ, Shizuka D, de Silva S, Waters JS, Prager SD, Sasaki T, Wittemyer G, Fewell J, McDonald DB (2014) The dynamics of animal social networks: analytical, conceptual, and theoretical advances. Behav Ecol 25(2):242–255.  https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/art047 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Royle NJ, Pike TW, Heeb P, Richner H, Kölliker M (2012) Offspring social network structure predicts fitness in families. Proc R Soc Lond B 279(1749):4914–4922.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2012.1701 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Silk JB (2003) Social bonds of female baboons enhance infant survival. Science 302(5648):1231–1234.  https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1088580 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Silk JB, Samuels A, Rodman PS (1981) The influence of kinship, rank, and sex on affiliation and aggression between adult female and immature bonnet macaques (Macaca radiata). Behaviour 78(1):111–137.  https://doi.org/10.1163/156853981X00284 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Silk JB, Beehner JC, Bergman TJ, Crockford C, Engh AL, Moscovice LR, Wittig RM, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (2010) Strong and consistent social bonds enhance the longevity of female baboons. Curr Biol 20(15):1359–1361.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2010.05.067 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Silk JB, Alberts SC, Altmann J, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM (2012) Stability of partner choice among female baboons. Anim Behav 83(6):1511–1518.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.03.028 PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Skaug H, Fournier D, Bolker BM, Nielsen A, Magnusson A (2016) Generalized linear mixed models using AD model builder. https://rdrr.io/rforge/glmmADMB/
  70. Smale L, Nunes S, Holekamp KE (1997) Sexually dimorphic dispersal in mammals: patterns, causes, and consequences. Adv Stud Behav 26:181–250.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-3454(08)60380-0 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Smith JE, Memenis SK, Holekamp KE (2007) Rank-related partner choice in the fission–fusion society of the spotted hyena (Crocuta crocuta). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 61(5):753–765.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-006-0305-y CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Smith JE, Kolowski JM, Graham KE, Dawes SE, Holekamp KE (2008) Social and ecological determinants of fission–fusion dynamics in the spotted hyaena. Anim Behav 76(3):619–636.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.05.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Smith JE, Powning KS, Dawes SE, Estrada JR, Hopper AL, Piotrowski SL, Holekamp KE (2011) Greetings promote cooperation and reinforce social bonds among spotted hyaenas. Anim Behav 81(2):401–415.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.11.007 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Stanton MA, Mann J (2012) Early social networks predict survival in wild bottlenose dolphins. PLoS One 7(10):e47508.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0047508 PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Stanton MA, Gibson QA, Mann J (2011) When mum’s away: a study of mother and calf ego networks during separations in wild bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp.) Anim Behav 82(2):405–412.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.05.026 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Suomi SJ (2005) Mother-infant attachment, peer relationships, and the development of social networks in rhesus monkeys. Hum Dev 48(1-2):67–79.  https://doi.org/10.1159/000083216 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Swanson EM, Dworkin I, Holekamp KE (2011) Lifetime selection on a hypoallometric size trait in the spotted hyena. Proc R Soc Lond B 278(1722):3277–3285.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2010.2512 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Swanson EM, McElhinny TL, Dworkin I, Weldele ML, Glickman SE, Holekamp KE (2013) Ontogeny of sexual size dimorphism in the spotted hyena (Crocuta crocuta). J Mammal 94(6):1298–1310.  https://doi.org/10.1644/12-MAMM-A-277.1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Szykman M, Engh AL, Van Horn RC, Funk S, Scribner K, Holekamp KE (2001) Association patterns among male and female spotted hyenas (Crocuta crocuta) reflect male mate choice. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 50(3):231–238.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s002650100356 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Tanner JB, Zelditch ML, Lundrigan BL, Holekamp KE (2009) Ontogenetic change in skull morphology and mechanical advantage in the spotted hyena (Crocuta crocuta). J Morphol 271:353–365Google Scholar
  81. Van Horn RC, McElhinny TL, Holekamp KE (2003) Age estimation and dispersal in the spotted hyena (Crocuta crocuta). J Mammal 84(3):1019–1030.  https://doi.org/10.1644/BBa-023 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. Vandeleest JJ, Beisner BA, Hannibal DL, Nathman AC, Capitanio JP, Hsieh F, Atwill ER, McCowan B (2016) Decoupling social status and status certainty effects on health in macaques: a network approach. PeerJ 4:e2394.  https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.2394 PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. Walker R, Burger O, Wagner J, Von Rueden CR (2006) Evolution of brain size and juvenile periods in primates. J Hum Evol 51(5):480–489.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhevol.2006.06.002 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. Watts HE, Tanner JB, Lundrigan BL, Holekamp KE (2009) Post-weaning maternal effects and the evolution of female dominance in the spotted hyena. Proc R Soc Lond B 276(1665):2291–2298.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2009.0268 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. Wilson A, Krause S, Dingemanse NJ, Krause J (2013) Network position: a key component in the characterization of social personality types. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 67(1):163–173.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-012-1428-y CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  • Julie W. Turner
    • 1
    • 2
  • Patrick S. Bills
    • 3
  • Kay E. Holekamp
    • 1
    • 2
  1. 1.Department of Integrative BiologyMichigan State UniversityEast LansingUSA
  2. 2.Ecology, Evolutionary Biology, and BehaviorMichigan State UniversityEast LansingUSA
  3. 3.Institute for Cyber-Enabled ResearchMichigan State UniversityEast LansingUSA

Personalised recommendations