Advertisement

Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology

, Volume 67, Issue 2, pp 321–330 | Cite as

Social organisation of thornbill-dominated mixed-species flocks using social network analysis

  • Damien R. Farine
  • Peter J. Milburn
Original Paper

Abstract

Mixed-species associations are a widespread phenomenon, comprising interacting heterospecific individuals which gain predator, foraging or social benefits. Avian flocks have traditionally been classified as monolithic species units, with species-wide functional roles, such as nuclear, active, passive, or follower. It has also been suggested that flocks are mutualistic interactions, where niches of participating species converge. However the species-level perspective has limited previous studies, because both interactions and benefits occur at the level of the individual. Social network analysis provides a set of tools for quantitative assessment of individual participation. We used mark-resighting methods to develop networks of nodes (colour-marked individuals) and edges (their interactions within flocks). We found that variation in flock participation across individuals within species, especially in the buff-rumped thornbill, encompassed virtually the entire range of variation across all individuals in the entire set of species. For example, female, but not male, buff-rumped thornbills had high network betweenness, indicating that they interact with multiple flocks, likely as part of a female-specific dispersal strategy. Finally, we provide new evidence that mixed-species flocking is mutualistic, by quantifying an active shift in individual foraging niches towards those of their individual associates, with implications for trade-off between costs and benefits to individuals derived from participating in mixed-species flocks. This study is, to our knowledge, the first instance of a heterospecific social network built on pairwise interactions.

Keywords

Social network analysis Mixed-species flock Group living Foraging niche Individual variation 

Notes

Acknowledgements

We wish to thank the Australian Capital Territory Government and Mulligan’s Flat park rangers (P. Mills, G. Woodbridge, et al.) for providing access and continued support and the Fenner School for Environment and Society at the Australian National University for their collaboration. We thank Dr. Andrew Reeson, Dr. Louise Tierney and Lucy Aplin for their assistance, and Prof. Ben Sheldon, Dr. Colin Garroway, Dr. Eben Goodale and one anonymous reviewer for insightful comments. Funding assistance was provided by the European Research Council (AdG 250164) awarded to B. Sheldon, and Wolfson College Fieldwork Grant awarded to DRF.

Supplementary material

265_2012_1452_MOESM1_ESM.kml (206 kb)
Online Resource 1 KML polygons showing and interactive overview of the study that can be viewed using Google Earth™. 1. Catching sites: two polygons showing the habitat patches in which all individuals were captured. 2. Overall home-range: home-range calculated from the observed locations of each group combined. 3. Individual home-range: home-range of each individual coloured by species and individually selectable with details of home-range size. (KML 205 kb).

References

  1. Bastian M, Heymann S (2010) Gephi: an open source software for exploring and manipulating networks, 0.8 alpha, https://gephi.org
  2. Bejder L, Fletcher D, Brager S (1998) A method for testing association patterns of social animals. Anim Behav 56:719–725PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bell HL (1980) Composition and seasonality of mixed-species feeding flocks of insectivorous birds in the Australian Capital Territory. Emu 80:227–232CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bell HL, Ford HA (1986) A comparison of the social-organization of 3 syntopic species of Australian Thornbill, Acanthiza. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 19:381–392CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Boinski S, Scott PE (1988) Association of birds with monkeys in Costa-Rica. Biotropica 20:136–143CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Buskirk WH (1976) Social-systems in a tropical forest Avifauna. Am Nat 110:293–310CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Butts CT (2010) sna: Tools for Social Network Analysis, R package version 2.1, http://erzuli.ss.uci.edu/R.stuff/
  8. Clutton-Brock T (2009) Cooperation between non-kin in animal societies. Nature 462:51–57PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Croft DP, James R, Thomas POR, Hathaway C, Mawdsley D, Laland KN, Krause J (2006) Social structure and co-operative interactions in a wild population of guppies (Poecilia reticulata). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 59:644–650CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Croft DP, James R, Krause J (2008) Exploring animal social networks. Princeton University Press, PrincetonGoogle Scholar
  11. Croft DP, Madden JR, Franks DW, James R (2011) Hypothesis testing in animal social networks. Trends Ecol Evol 26:502–507PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. del Hoyo J, Elliott A, Christie DA (eds) (2007) Handbook of the birds of the world. Vol. 12. Picathartes to Tits and Chickadees. Lynx Edicions, BarcelonaGoogle Scholar
  13. Dhondt AA (2012) Interspecific competition. Oxford Avian Biology Series. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  14. Diamond JM (1975) Assembly of species communities. In: Cody ML, Diamond JM (eds) Ecology and evolution of communities. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, pp 342–444Google Scholar
  15. Dolby AS, Grubb TC (1998) Benefits to satellite members in mixed-species foraging groups: an experimental analysis. Anim Behav 56:501–509PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Dolby AS, Grubb TC (2000) Social context affects risk taking by a satellite species in a mixed-species foraging group. Behav Ecol 11:110–114CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Evans PGH (1982) Associations between seabirds and cetaceans — a review. Mammal Rev 12:187–206CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Farley EA, Sieving KE, Contreras TA (2008) Characterizing complex mixed-species bird flocks using an objective method for determining species participation. J Ornithol 149:451–468CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Franks DW, Ruxton GD, James R (2010) Sampling animal association networks with the gambit of the group. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 64:493–503CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Goodale E, Kotagama SW (2005a) Alarm calling in Sri Lankan mixed-species bird flocks. Auk 122:108–120CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Goodale E, Kotagama SW (2005b) Testing the roles of species in mixed-species bird flocks of a Sri Lankan rain forest. J Trop Ecol 21:669–676CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Goodale E, Kotagama SW (2008) Response to conspecific and heterospecific alarm calls in mixed-species bird flocks of a Sri Lankan rainforest. Behav Ecol 19:887–894CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Goodale E, Beauchamp G, Magrath RD, Nieh JC, Ruxton GD (2010) Interspecific information transfer influences animal community structure. Trends Ecol Evol 25:354–361PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Graves GR, Gotelli NJ (1993) Assembly of avian mixed-species flocks in Amazonia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 90:1388–1391PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Greenberg R (2000) Birds of many feathers: the formation and structure of mixed-species flocks of forest birds. In: Boinski S, Gerber PA (eds) On the move: how and why animals travel in groups. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 521–558Google Scholar
  26. Harrison NM, Whitehouse MJ (2011) Mixed-species flocks: an example of niche construction? Anim Behav 81:675–682CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Hartigan PM (1985) Computation of the dip statistic to test for unimodality. Appl Stat J Roy St C 34:320–325Google Scholar
  28. Hartigan JA, Hartigan PM (1985) The dip test of unimodality. Ann Stat 13:70–84CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Hinde RA (1976) Interactions, relationships and social-structure. Man 11:1–17CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Hino T (1998) Mutualistic and commensal organization of avian mixed-species foraging flocks in a forest of western Madagascar. J Avian Biol 29:17–24CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Hino T (2000) Intraspecific differences in benefits from feeding in mixed-species flocks. J Avian Biol 31:441–446CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Hino T (2002) Breeding bird community and mixed-species flocking in a deciduous broad-leaved forest in western Madagascar. Ornithol Sci 1:111–116CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Horne JS, Garton EO, Krone SM, Lewis JS (2007) Analyzing animal movements using Brownian bridges. Ecology 88:2354–2363PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Hsieh FS, Chen CC (2011) Does niche-overlap facilitate mixed-species flocking in birds? J Ornithol 152:955–963CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Hutchinson GE (1957) Population studies—animal ecology and demography—concluding remarks. Cold Spring Harb Sym 22:415–427CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Hutto RL (1994) The composition and social-organization of mixed-species flocks in a tropical deciduous forest in Western Mexico. Condor 96:105–118CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Krause J, Ruxton GD (2002) Living in groups. Oxford Series in Ecology and Environment. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  38. Krause J, Croft DP, James R (2007) Social network theory in the behavioural sciences: potential applications. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 62:15–27CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Lack DL (1971) Ecological isolation in birds. Harvard University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  40. Latta SC, Wunderle JM (1996) The composition and foraging ecology of mixed-species flocks in pine forests of Hispaniola. Condor 98:595–607CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Lepschi BJ (1993) Vegetation of Mulligans Flat, A.C.T. Cunninghamia 3:155–166Google Scholar
  42. Magrath RD, Pitcher BJ, Gardner JL (2007) A mutual understanding? Interspecific responses by birds to each other’s aerial alarm calls. Behav Ecol 18:944–951CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Magrath RD, Pitcher BJ, Gardner JL (2009) An avian eavesdropping network: alarm signal reliability and heterospecific response. Behav Ecol 20:745–752CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Maldonado-Coelho M, Marini MA (2004) Mixed-species bird flocks from Brazilian Atlantic forest: the effects of forest fragmentation and seasonality on their size, richness and stability. Biol Conserv 116:19–26CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Manning AD, Wood JT, Cunningham RB, McIntyre S, Shorthouse DJ, Gordon IJ, Lindenmayer DB (2011) Integrating research and restoration: the establishment of a long-term woodland experiment in south-eastern Australia. Aust Zool 35:633–648Google Scholar
  46. Martin TE (1996) Fitness costs of resource overlap among coexisting bird species. Nature 380:338–340CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. McDonald DB (2007) Predicting fate from early connectivity in a social network. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104:10910–10914PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. McIntyre S, Stol J, Harvey J, Nicholls AO, Campbell M, Reid A, Manning AD, Lindenmeyer D (2010) Biomass and floristic patterns in the ground layer vegetation of box-gum grassy eucalypt woodland in Goorooyarroo and Mulligans Flat Nature Reserves, Australian Capital Territory. Cunninghamia 11:319–357Google Scholar
  49. Morse DH (1970) Ecological aspects of some mixed-species foraging flocks of birds. Ecol Monogr 40:119–168CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Moynihan M (1962) The organization and probable evolution of some mixed species flocks of neotropical birds. Smithson Misc Coll 143:1–140Google Scholar
  51. Newman MEJ (2003) Mixing patterns in networks. Phys Rev E 67Google Scholar
  52. Newman MEJ (2010) Networks: an introduction. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  53. Nicholls JA, Double MC, Rowell DM, Magrath RD (2000) The evolution of cooperative and pair breeding in thornbills Acanthiza (Pardalotidae). J Avian Biol 31:165–176CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Oh KP, Badyaev AV (2010) Structure of social networks in a Passerine bird: consequences for sexual selection and the evolution of mating strategies. Am Nat 176:E80–E89PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Oommen MA, Shanker K (2010) Shrewd alliances: mixed foraging associations between treeshrews, greater racket-tailed drongos and sparrowhawks on Great Nicobar Island, India. Biol Lett 6:304–307PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Powell GVN (1985) Sociobiology and adaptive significance of interspecific foraging flocks in the Neotropics. Ornithol Monogr 36:713–732CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. R Development Core Team (2010) R: a language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria, http://www.R-project.org
  58. Schmidt KA, Dall SRX, van Gils JA (2010) The ecology of information: an overview on the ecological significance of making informed decisions. Oikos 119:304–316CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Schoener TW (1974) Resource partitioning in ecological communities. Science 185:27–39PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Schoener TW (1983) Field experiments on interspecific competition. Am Nat 122:240–285CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Sridhar H, Beauchamp G, Shanker K (2009) Why do birds participate in mixed-species foraging flocks? A large-scale synthesis. Anim Behav 78:337–347CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Srinivasan U, Raza RH, Quader S (2010) The nuclear question: rethinking species importance in multi-species animal groups. J Anim Ecol 79:948–954PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Sundaresan SR, Fischhoff IR, Dushoff J (2009) Avoiding spurious findings of nonrandom social structure in association data. Anim Behav 77:1381–1385CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Szekely T, Szep T, Juhasz T (1989) Mixed species flocking of tits (Parus spp.) — a field experiment. Oecologia 78:490–495CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Thiollay JM (1999) Frequency of mixed species flocking in tropical forest birds and correlates of predation risk: an intertropical comparison. J Avian Biol 30:282–294CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Thiollay JM (2003) Comparative foraging behavior between solitary and flocking insectivores in a Neotropical forest: does vulnerability matter? Ornithol Neotrop 14:47–65Google Scholar
  67. Thiollay JM, Jullien M (1998) Flocking behaviour of foraging birds in a neotropical rain forest and the antipredator defence hypothesis. Ibis 140:382–394CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Valburg LK (1992) Flocking and frugivory — the effect of social groupings on resource use in the common bush-tanager. Condor 94:358–363CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Waite TA, Grubb TC (1988) Copying of foraging locations in mixed-species flocks of temperate-deciduous woodland birds — an experimental-study. Condor 90:132–140CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Wascher CAF, Szipl G, Boeckle M, Wilkinson A (2012) You sound familiar: carrion crows can differentiate between the calls of known and unknown heterospecifics. Anim Cogn 15:1015–1019PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Whitehead H (1995) Investigating structure and temporal scale in social organizations using identified individuals. Behav Ecol 6:199–208CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Whitehead H (2008) Analyzing animal societies. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  73. Whitehead H, Bejder L, Ottensmeyer CA (2005) Testing association patterns: issues arising and extensions. Anim Behav 69:e1–e6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Winterbottom JM (1943) On woodland bird parties in northern Rhodesia. Ibis 85:437–442CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Zou FS, Chen GZ, Yang QF, Fellowes JR (2011) Composition of mixed-species flocks and shifts in foraging location of flocking species on Hainan Island, China. Ibis 153:269–278CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2012

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Edward Grey Institute of Field Ornithology, Department of ZoologyUniversity of OxfordOxfordUK
  2. 2.College of Medicine, Biology & EnvironmentAustralian National UniversityCanberraAustralia

Personalised recommendations