Advertisement

Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology

, Volume 66, Issue 2, pp 223–230 | Cite as

Social facilitation of exploration in mosquitofish (Gambusia holbrooki)

  • Ashley J. W. Ward
Original Paper

Abstract

The social environment can exert a powerful influence on the expression of an individual’s behaviour patterns. For example, social facilitation occurs where individuals are more likely to express a given behaviour, or express it a greater rate, in the presence of conspecifics. Social facilitation is partly driven by an individual’s perception of risk, which is a function both of the size of its social group and the information that it gathers relating to predator activity and risk. Here I tested the effects of social group size (one, two, four, eight or 16 fish) and the presence of ‘social’, ‘predation’ or ‘neutral’ chemical cues (derived respectively from live conspecifics, injured conspecifics or a blank water control) on the exploratory behaviour of juvenile mosquitofish (Gambusia holbrooki) in a novel environment. Focal fish in larger groups explored a greater proportion of the arena during the course of the experiment, demonstrating social facilitation of exploration. After 4 h in the arena, focal fish in all group sizes showed significantly reduced swimming activity, suggesting that the initial faster swimming activity of fish on entry to the arena may be in response to the motivation to explore. The presence of predation cues in the environment had the effect of reducing exploratory behaviour across groups in a novel environment, whereas social cues had no effect on exploratory behaviour. Taken as a whole, these results suggest that there is a high degree of context dependency in the expression of exploratory behaviour, with a strong influence of both the presence of conspecifics and cues relating to potential danger.

Keywords

Sociality Shoaling Social information Alarm cues 

References

  1. Aeschlimann PB, Haberli MA, Reusch TBH, Boehm T, Milinski M (2003) Female sticklebacks Gasterosteus aculeatus use self-reference to optimize MHC allele number during mate selection. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 54:119–126Google Scholar
  2. Baird TA, Ryer CH, Olla BL (1991) Social enhancement of foraging on an ephemeral food source in juvenile walleye pollock, Theragra chalcogramma. Env Biol Fish 31:307–311CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Barata EN, Hubbard PC, Almeida OG, Miranda A, Canario AVM (2007) Male urine signals social rank in the Mozambique tilapia (Oreochromis mossambicus). BMC Biol 5:54PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Basil J, Sandeman D (2000) Crayfish (Cherax destructor) use tactile cues to detect and learn topographical changes in their environment. Ethology 106:247–259CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Brown GE, Bongiorno T, DiCapua DM, Ivan LI, Roh E (2006) Effects of group size on the threat-sensitive response to varying concentrations of chemical alarm cues by juvenile convict cichlids. Can J Zool 84:1–8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Burns JG (2008) The validity of three tests of temperament in guppies (Poecilia reticulata). J Comp Psychol 122:344–356PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Burt de Perera T (2004) Fish can encode order in their spatial map. 2004. Proceedings of the Royal Society B 271:2131–2134PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Crusio WE (2001) Genetic dissection of mouse exploratory behaviour. Behav Brain Res 125:127–132PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Dally JM, Clayton NS, Emery NJ (2008) Social influences on foraging by rooks (Corvus frugilegus). Behaviour 145:1101–1124CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Day RL, MacDonald T, Brown C, Laland KN, Reader SM (2001) Interactions between shoal size and conformity in guppy social foraging. Anim Behav 62:917–925CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Dingemanse NJ, Kazem AJN, Reale D, Wright J (2010) Behavioural reaction norms: animal personality meets individual plasticity. Trends Ecol Evol 25:81–89PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Ferrari MCO, Wisenden BD, Chivers DP (2010) Chemical ecology of predator–prey interactions in aquatic ecosystems: a review and prospectus. Can J Zool 88:698–724CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Grand TC, Dill LM (1999) The effect of group size on the foraging behaviour of juvenile coho salmon: reduction of predation risk or increased competition? Anim Behav 58:443–451PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Griffiths RA, Foster JP (1998) The effect of social interactions on tadpole activity and growth in the British anuran amphibians (Bufo bufo, B. calamita, and Rana temporaria). J Zool 245:431–437CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Hager MC, Helfman GS (1991) Safety in numbers—shoal size choice by minnows under predatory threat. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 29:271–276CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Hasler AD, Cooper JC (1976) Chemical cues for homing salmon. Experientia 32:1091–1093PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Herbert-Read JE, Logendran D, Ward AJW (2010) Sensory ecology in a changing world: salinity alters conspecific recognition in an amphidromous fish, Pseudomugil signifer. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 64:1107–1115CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Hoare DJ, Couzin ID, Godin JGJ, Krause J (2004) Context-dependent group size choice in fish. Anim Behav 67:155–164CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Kats LB, Dill LM (1998) The scent of death: Chemosensory assessment of predation risk by prey animals. Ecoscience 5:361–394Google Scholar
  20. Keenleyside MHA (1955) Some aspects of the schooling behaviour of fish. Behaviour 8:83–248CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Kleerekoper H, Matis J, Gensler P, Maynard P (1974) Exploratory behavior of goldfish Carassius auratus. Anim Behav 22:124–132CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Magnhagen C, Bunnefeld N (2009) Express your personality or go along with the group: what determines the behaviour of shoaling perch? Proc R Soc Lond B 276:3369–3375CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Magnhagen C, Staffan F (2005) Is boldness affected by group composition in young-of-the-year perch (Perca fluviatilis)? Behav Ecol Sociobiol 57:295–303CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Magurran AE, Pitcher TJ (1983) Foraging, timidity and shoal size in minnows and goldfish. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 12:147–152CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Meuthen D, Baldauf SA, Bakker TCM, Thünken T (2011) Substrate-treated water: a method to enhance fish activity in laboratory experiments. Aquat Biol 13:35–40. doi: 10.3354/ab00348 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Mikheev VN, Andreev OA (1993) 2-Phase exploration of a novel environment in the guppy, Poecilia reticulata. J Fish Biol 42:375–383CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Oosten JE, Magnhagen C, Hemelrijk CK (2010) Boldness by habituation and social interactions: a model. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 64:793–802PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Pyke GH (2005) A review of the biology of Gambusia affinis and G-holbrooki. Rev Fish Biol Fish 15:339–365CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Schuett W, Dall SRX (2009) Sex differences, social context and personality in zebra finches, Taeniopygia guttata. Anim Behav 77:1041–1050CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Suarez SD, Gallup GG (1982) Open-field behavior in chickens—the experimenter is a predator. J Comp Physiol Psychol 96:432–439CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. van Oers K, Klunder M, Drent PJ (2005) Context dependence of personalities: risk-taking behavior in a social and a nonsocial situation. Behav Ecol 16:716–723CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Walsh RN, Cummins RA (1976) The open-field test—a critical-review. Psychol Bull 83:482–504PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Ward AJW, Mehner T (2010) Multimodal mixed messages: the use of multiple cues allows greater accuracy in social recognition and predator detection decisions in the mosquitofish, Gambusia holbrooki. Behav Ecol 21:1315–1320CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Ward AJW, Webster MM, Hart PJB (2007) Social recognition in wild fish populations. Proc Royal Soc Lond B 274:1071–1077CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Ward AJW, Sumpter DJT, Couzin LD, Hart PJB, Krause J (2008) Quorum decision-making facilitates information transfer in fish shoals. P Natl Acad Sci USA 105:6948–6953CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Ward AJW, Herbert-Read JE, Sumpter DJT, Krause J (2011) Fast and accurate decisions through collective vigilance in fish shoals. P Natl Acad Sci USA 108:2312–2315CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Webster MM, Hart PJB (2006) Subhabitat selection by foraging threespine stickleback (Gasterosteus aculeatus): previous experience and social conformity. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 60:77–86CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Webster MM, Ward AJW (2010) Personality and social context. Biological Reviews. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-185X.2010.00169.x PubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Webster MM, Atton N, Ward AJW, Hart PJB (2007a) Turbidity and foraging rate in threespine sticklebacks: the importance of visual and chemical prey cues. Behaviour 144:1347–1360CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Webster MM, Goldsmith J, Ward AJW, Hart PJB (2007b) Habitat-specific chemical cues influence association preferences and shoal cohesion in fish. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 62:273–280CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Webster MM, Ward AJW, Hart PJB (2007c) Boldness is influenced by social context in threespine sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus). Behaviour 144:351–371CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Welker WI, Welker J (1958) Reaction of fish (Eucinostomus gula) to environmental-changes. Ecology 39:283–288CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Whimbey AE, Denenber VH (1967) Two independent behavioral dimensions in open-field performance. J Comp Physiol Psychol 63:500–504PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. White JW, Warner RR (2007) Behavioral and energetic costs of group membership in a coral reef fish. Oecologia 154:423–433PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Wisenden BD (2000) Olfactory assessment of predation risk in the aquatic environment. Phil Trans Roy Soc Lond B 355:1205–1208CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Wisenden BD, Binstock CL, Knoll KE, Linke AD, Demuth BS (2010) Risk-sensitive information gathering by cyprinids following release of chemical alarm cues. Anim Behav 79:1101–1107CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Zajonc RB (1965) Social facilitation. Science 149:269–274PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2011

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.School of Biological SciencesUniversity of SydneySydneyAustralia

Personalised recommendations